Perceived gloss was plotted as a function of specular roughness for each of the levels of relief height and viewing orientation (
Figure 9). Separate rows of axes show data obtained for surfaces having each of the three different diffuse colors. We analyzed the results separately for each of the nine conditions below.
Red surfaces were analyzed for effects on perceived gloss at each viewing orientation using separate repeated-measures ANOVAs. For the 15° condition, there was a significant main effect of specular roughness, F(3, 9) = 244.2, p < 0.00001, and relief height, F(3, 9) = 281.7, p < 0.0005. There was also a significant interaction effect between specular roughness and relief height on perceived gloss, F(9, 27) = 77.44, p < 0.0005. For the 30° condition, there was a significant main effect of specular roughness, F(3, 9) = 320.6, p < 0.00001, and relief height, F(3, 9) = 24.52, p < 0.0005. There was also a significant interaction effect between specular roughness and relief height on perceived gloss, F(9, 27) = 5.65, p < 0.0005. For the 45° condition, there was a significant main effect of specular roughness, F(3, 9) = 180.7, p < 0.00001, and relief height, F(3, 9) = 9.01, p < 0.005. There was also a significant interaction effect between specular roughness and relief height on perceived gloss, F(9, 27) = 5.65, p < 0.0005.
Next, green surfaces were analyzed for effects on perceived gloss at each viewing orientation using separate repeated-measures ANOVAs. For the 15° condition, there was a significant main effect of specular roughness, F(3, 9) = 114.8, p < 0.00001, and relief height F(3, 9) = 165.1, p < 0.00001. There was also a significant interaction effect between specular roughness and relief height on perceived gloss, F(9, 27) = 32.97, p < 0.00001. For the 30° condition, there was a significant main effect of specular roughness, F(3, 9) = 159.9, p < 0.00001, and relief height, F(3, 9) = 13.88, p < 0.005. However, there was no significant interaction effect between specular roughness and relief height on perceived gloss, F(9, 27) = 1.22, p < 0.32. For the 45° condition, there was a significant main effect of specular roughness, F(3, 9) = 693, p < 0.00001, and relief height, F(3, 9) = 24.29, p < 0.0005. There was also a significant interaction effect between specular roughness and relief height on perceived gloss, F(9, 27) = 4.47, p < 0.005.
Finally, blue surfaces were analyzed for effects on perceived gloss at each viewing orientation using separate repeated-measures ANOVAs. For the 15° condition, there was a significant main effect of specular roughness, F(3, 9) = 438, p < 0.00001, and relief height, F(3, 9) = 165.4, p < 0.00001. There was also a significant interaction effect between specular roughness and relief height on perceived gloss, F(9, 27) = 70.25, p < 0.00001. For the 30° condition, there was a significant main effect of specular roughness, F(3, 9) = 169.2, p < 0.00001, and relief height, F(3, 9) = 33.22, p < 0.00005. However, there was no significant interaction effect between specular roughness and relief height on perceived gloss, F(9, 27) = 2.07, p < 0.07. For the 45° condition, there was a significant main effect of specular roughness, F(3, 9) = 404.1, p < 0.00001, and relief height, F(3, 9) = 5.89, p < 0.05. There was also a significant interaction effect between specular roughness and relief height on perceived gloss, F(9, 27) = 7.37, p < 0.00005.
We found the same consistent decline in perceived gloss with increasing specular roughness, especially for surfaces oriented more frontally to the observer and obliquely relative to the primary lighting direction, which other studies have reported (
Marlow et al., 2012;
Honson et al., 2020). We also found the same pattern of results as
Honson et al. (2020) in that the effect of varying relief height on perceived gloss was more variable for surfaces that were oriented frontally (i.e., 15°). This can be observed in
Figure 10, where there is larger displacement between curves plotted for the 15° condition compared to the increasingly slanted surfaces across all hues. An inverse pattern of responses across specular roughness values can also be observed for the lowest relief height value (0.025) across all hues (
Figure 9); increases in perceived gloss were observed with increasing specular roughness values that asymptote between values 0.300 and 0.400. We found very little difference in the effects of relief height and specular roughness on perceived gloss across different hues. These results suggest that perceived gloss alone cannot account for the differences we observed in
Experiment 1 for perceived lightness and saturation.