Participants were well able to maintain their fixation and execute the required eye movements (
Supplementary Figure S1). The mean distances between eye position and fixation center were 0.11° (
SD = 0.09°) and 0.36° (
SD = 0.28°) before and after saccades. Saccades, which need to be executed within 500 ms after the extinction of the left fixation cross, were on average accurate and prompt, with 143.6 ms (
SD = 117.3) mean saccade latency. In only 1.15% of all trials, the saccades were not executed before the test stimulus presentation. Because of the very small proportion of these delayed saccades, our results were not affected by whether we exclude these trials or not in the following statistical analysis.
For participants who finished separate sessions with and without eye movement recording, no significant differences were found between the two sessions (dependent sample t-tests for all conditions,
p > 0.05,
Supplementary Figure S2). There were also no significant differences between participants with and without eye movement recording (independent sample
t-tests for all conditions,
p > 0.05,
Supplementary Figure S3). Thus we combined these data in the further statistical analysis.
The strength of TAE and FGAE for each participant was calculated as half of the difference on the x-axis between the two points of subjective equality (PSEs) based on the psychometric functions following adaptation in two opposite orientation (TAE) or gender (FGAE) (see
Figure 2 for an example). Statistics were then performed on the group data.
We performed two-way ANOVAs to examine the effects of two factors (two levels of adaptor awareness and five different adapt-test relationships) on the magnitude of TAE and FGAE. For the TAE, both the main effects of adaptor awareness and adapt-test relationship are significant (adaptor awareness: F(1,11) = 61.48, p < 0.001, \(\eta _p^2 = 0.848\); adapt-test relationship: F(4, 44) = 61.61, p < 0.001, \(\eta _p^2 = 0.849\)). The interaction between adaptor awareness and the adapt-test relationship was also significant (F(4, 44) = 11.71, p < 0.001, \(\eta _p^2 = 0.516\)), indicating that the impact of adaptor awareness depended on the relationship between adapt-test locations. Post hoc analysis showed that the TAE in spatiotopic location is significantly larger than the control-spatiotopic location in both visible (t = 5.91, p < 0.001) and invisible condition (t = 3.26, p < 0.01), suggesting the existence of a spatially specific adaptation effect at the spatiotopic location, regardless of awareness state of the adapting stimulus.
For the FGAE, again both the main effects of adaptor awareness and adapt-test relationship are significant (adaptor awareness: F(1, 11) = 14.49, p = 0.003, \(\eta _p^2 = 0.568\); adapt-test relationship: F(4, 44) = 12.15, p < 0.001, \(\eta _p^2 = 0.525\)). However, the interaction effect between adaptor awareness and adapt-test relationship is not significant (F(4, 44) = 1.83, p = 0.141, \(\eta _p^2 = 0.142\)), suggesting that the impact of adaptor awareness was not dependent on the relationship between adapt-test locations. Post hoc analysis showed that the FGAE in spatiotopic location is not significantly larger than that in the control-spatiotopic location in both visible and invisible conditions (p > 0.05).
For the visible condition (without CFS), the one-sample t-tests with Holm correction (
N = 10, 5 locations* 2 state awareness (with(out) CFS) for TAE and FGAE respectively) indicate that both TAE and FGAE could be induced at the spatiotopic location (TAE:
M = 0.93°,
p < 0.001; FGAE:
M = 7.56%,
p < 0.001), and not surprisingly, at the retinotopic location (TAE:
M = 2.26°,
p < 0.001; FGAE:
M = 16.67%,
p < 0.001). Results show that the TAE and FGAE partially transfer to control-retinotopic location (TAE:
M = 0.48°,
p < 0.01; FGAE:
M = 5.46%,
p < 0.05) and control-spatiotopic location (TAE:
M = 0.27°,
p < 0.05; FGAE:
M = 7.85%,
p < 0.01). The full adaptation condition (no saccade) reveals the strength of the TAE (
M = 2.38°,
p < 0.001) and FGAE (
M = 10.31%,
p < 0.001) in the classic condition (
Figure 3, left panels) (also see normalized results in
Supplementary Figure S4).
For the invisible condition (with CFS), interestingly, results show that both stimuli could still generate robust aftereffects at the retinotopic (TAE:
M = 0.85°,
p < 0.02; FGAE:
M = 6.62%,
p < 0.02) and spatiotopic locations (TAE:
M = 0.25°,
p < 0.02; FGAE:
M = 3.88%,
p < 0.03), whereas no aftereffect was observed at the control-spatiotopic location (TAE:
M = 0.02°,
p = 0.97; FGAE:
M = 1.09%,
p = 0.88) nor at the control-retinotopic location (TAE:
M = 0.02°,
p = 0 .97; FGAE:
M = 0 .19%,
p = 0.88). For the full adaptation condition without saccade, significant TAE and FGAE were observed (TAE:
M = 0.69°,
p < 0.01; FGAE:
M = 6.83%,
p < 0.05) (
Figure 3, right panels). Comparing with results in the visible adaptation condition, the spread of aftereffects to control locations did not occur when participants had no awareness of the adaptation stimulus; however, the adaptation effect remained robust at the spatiotopic location.