Open Access
Article  |   May 2021
Dependence of perceptual saccadic suppression on peri-saccadic image flow properties and luminance contrast polarity
Author Affiliations
  • Matthias P. Baumann
    Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, Tübingen University, Tübingen, Germany
    Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Tübingen University, Tübingen, Germany
    matthias-philipp.baumann@student.uni-tuebingen.de
  • Saad Idrees
    Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, Tübingen University, Tübingen, Germany
    idrees.sa@gmail.com
  • Thomas A. Münch
    Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, Tübingen University, Tübingen, Germany
    thomas.muench@gmail.com
  • Ziad M. Hafed
    Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, Tübingen University, Tübingen, Germany
    Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Tübingen University, Tübingen, Germany
    ziad.m.hafed@cin.uni-tuebingen.de
Journal of Vision May 2021, Vol.21, 15. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.5.15
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Matthias P. Baumann, Saad Idrees, Thomas A. Münch, Ziad M. Hafed; Dependence of perceptual saccadic suppression on peri-saccadic image flow properties and luminance contrast polarity. Journal of Vision 2021;21(5):15. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.5.15.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Across saccades, perceptual detectability of brief visual stimuli is strongly diminished. We recently observed that this perceptual suppression phenomenon is jumpstarted in the retina, suggesting that the phenomenon might be significantly more visual in nature than normally acknowledged. Here, we explicitly compared saccadic suppression strength when saccades were made across a uniform image of constant luminance versus when saccades were made across image patches of different luminance, width, and trans-saccadic luminance polarity. We measured perceptual contrast thresholds of human subjects for brief peri-saccadic flashes of positive (luminance increments) or negative (luminance decrements) polarity. Thresholds were >6–7 times higher when saccades translated a luminance stripe or edge across the retina than when saccades were made over a completely uniform image patch. Critically, both background luminance and flash luminance polarity strongly modulated peri-saccadic contrast thresholds. In addition, all of these very same visual dependencies also occurred in the absence of any saccades, but with qualitatively similar rapid translations of image patches across the retina. This similarity of visual dependencies with and without saccades supports the notion that perceptual saccadic suppression may be fundamentally a visual phenomenon, which strongly motivates neurophysiological and theoretical investigations on the role of saccadic eye movement commands in modulating its properties.

Introduction
Due to their ballistic nature, visual input across saccades invariably includes periods of large image uncertainty. Perceptual cancelation during such periods takes place, resulting in a seamless subjective visual experience despite the occurrence of saccades as often as several times in just one second (Melcher, 2011; Wurtz, 2008; Wurtz, Joiner, & Berman, 2011; Zimmermann & Bremmer, 2016). In the laboratory, the properties of peri-saccadic vision have been studied by presenting very brief and fleeting “probe” stimuli around the time of saccadic eye movements. Such stimuli act as impulses that essentially capture the instantaneous and momentary state of the visual system (Matin, 1974; Ross, Morrone, Goldberg, & Burr, 2001). 
When presenting very brief peri-saccadic visual stimuli, a prominent observation is a massive, but transient, suppression of visual sensitivity. In this striking phenomenon, a visual probe goes completely unnoticed if presented within approximately ± 50 ms from saccade onset, even if it would be detected effortlessly when presented under fixation (Beeler, 1967; Brooks & Fuchs, 1975; Idrees, Baumann, Franke, Munch, & Hafed, 2020; Latour, 1962). This phenomenon was labeled saccadic suppression (Zuber & Stark, 1966), and it has attracted investigation by vision scientists for many decades (Binda & Morrone, 2018; Castet, Jeanjean, & Masson, 2001; Castet & Masson, 2000; Matin, 1974; Schweitzer & Rolfs, 2020; Wurtz, 2008). Saccadic suppression is robust, and it occurs for both reflexive and deliberate saccades (Gremmler & Lappe, 2017). It also occurs for saccades of all sizes, including microsaccades (Beeler, 1967; Chen & Hafed, 2017; Hafed & Krauzlis, 2010; Hass & Horwitz, 2011; Scholes, McGraw, & Roach, 2018; Zuber & Stark, 1966), and it even acts to shape the long-term dynamics of visual sensitivity well after the eye movements (J. Bellet, Chen, & Hafed, 2017; Benedetto & Morrone, 2017). Most interestingly, neural responses to visual probes in a variety of brain areas are also suppressed if the probes occur peri-saccadically (Berman, Cavanaugh, McAlonan, & Wurtz, 2017; Bremmer, Kubischik, Hoffmann, & Krekelberg, 2009; Chen & Hafed, 2017; Chen, Ignashchenkova, Thier, & Hafed, 2015; Hafed & Krauzlis, 2010; Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020; Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020; Kagan, Gur, & Snodderly, 2008; Reppas, Usrey, & Reid, 2002; Robinson & Wurtz, 1976). 
For perhaps as long as saccadic suppression has been investigated, there have been debates on its origins. On the one hand, according to the “active hypothesis,” it has been suggested that suppression relies on knowledge of saccade generation commands to actively suppress visual sensitivity, through efference copies or corollary discharge from (pre-) motor areas (Beeler, 1967; Diamond, Ross, & Morrone, 2000; Duffy & Lombroso, 1968; Zuber & Stark, 1966). Perceptually, a showpiece for this hypothesis was the observation that saccadic suppression can be selective (Burr, Morrone, & Ross, 1994): brief flashes of low spatial frequency patterns are suppressed much more than brief flashes of high spatial frequency patterns (Burr et al., 1994). This observation was interpreted as evidence for an active movement-related signal specifically targeting the magnocellular visual pathway, which is sensitive to low spatial frequencies (DeYoe & Van Essen, 1988; Merigan & Maunsell, 1993). On the other hand, according to the “visual hypothesis,” it is the visual consequences of saccades that jumpstart suppression. This is supported by the observation that brief probe flashes near the onset of global image translations (similar to those caused by saccades) are also perceptually suppressed (Adey & Noda, 1973; Brooks & Fuchs, 1975; Brooks, Impelman, & Lum, 1981; Castet et al., 2001; Diamond et al., 2000; Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020; Mackay, 1970; Mitrani, Mateeff, & Yakimoff, 1971; Mitrani, Radil-Weiss, Yakimoff, Mateeff, & Bozkov, 1975; Mitrani, Yakimoff, & Mateeff, 1973; Noda & Adey, 1974; Yakimoff, Mitrani, & Mateeff, 1974). Moreover, such suppression still takes place when the flashes are presented before the image translations, likely through backward masking effects (Brooks et al., 1981; Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020; Macknik & Livingstone, 1998). Recently, we also found that even selective suppression of low spatial frequency patterns may be entirely visual in origin: both selective and unselective suppression can happen with or without saccades, simply as a function of visual context (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). A key observation was that saccade-like image translations activate, rather than suppress, the retinal image processing cascade and result in afferent activity bursts in retinal ganglion cells; responses to subsequent flashes are, in turn, suppressed through visually-triggered retinal-circuit (and downstream) mechanisms (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020; Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020). Thus, from a visual perspective, perceptual saccadic suppression involves visual-visual interactions between two kinds of signals: (1) the visual consequences of saccadic eyeball rotations; and (2) the visual consequences of flash onsets (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). 
Here, we explored these visual-visual interactions in more detail at the perceptual level. We varied the image conditions across which gaze translated during saccades, and we also tested image translations in the absence of saccades. We additionally explored influences of flash polarity (luminance increments or decrements relative to the background), motivated by quantitative differences between ON and OFF retinal pathways in saccadic suppression (Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020). We found that saccadic suppression exhibits a large diversity of visual dependencies, which also emerge with image translations in the absence of saccades. Our results motivate revisiting both the movement-related and visual components of saccadic suppression and investigating how saccade movement commands may interact with visual-visual interactions in shaping trans-saccadic visual perception. 
Methods
Subjects and ethical approvals
We collected data from six human subjects (three females) who provided informed consent. Two subjects were authors (MPB and SI); the others were naïve to the purposes of the experiments. The subjects (aged 22–32 years) were compensated 10 euros per 1-hour session. Each subject's data were collected across 10 sessions. The experiments were approved by ethics committees at the Medical Faculty of Tübingen University, and they were in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. 
Laboratory setup
The setup was similar to that described in recent studies (Bogadhi, Buonocore, & Hafed, 2020; Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). Subjects sat 57 cm from a CRT display (41 pixels/degrees; 85 Hz) spanning approximately 34 degrees horizontally and 26 degrees vertically. We stabilized subjects’ heads using a custom-built device (Hafed, 2013). In addition, we tracked movements of the left eye using a desktop-mounted video-based eye tracker (Eyelink 1000, SR Research Ltd, Canada). Before running the experiments, we linearized the display (8-bit resolution), such that we had equal steps of luminance increments or decrements (relative to the background luminance) for the different experimental variants (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). All experiments were controlled using the Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997; Kleiner, Brainard, & Pelli, 2007; Pelli, 1997), with Eyelink extensions (Cornelissen, Peters, & Palmer, 2002). 
Experimental procedures
Our general approach was to present a brief probe flash (one display frame; approximately 12 ms) at a specific time relative to a visual transient. In some conditions, such a visual transient was caused by a real saccade shifting the retinal image globally. In other conditions, it was caused by either a luminance step in the display during fixation or by a transient translation of the display image (to simulate a saccade-like image displacement). The probe was presented at one of multiple possible display locations, which were fixed in position for all experiments. Subjects had to identify, on every trial, at which of the locations the probe flash appeared. Therefore, the experiment utilized a multiple-alternative forced-choice design. If subjects’ perception was suppressed, then their proportion of correct responses was expected to significantly decrease relative to normal performance. 
To obtain a sensitive measure of perceptual sensitivity across the different conditions, we varied the flash contrast (luminance amplitude, either as an increment or decrement from background luminance) across all trials, and we collected full psychometric curves of perceptual detectability. This allowed us to calculate perceptual thresholds (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). 
Across 10 sessions per subject, we tested five different visual transient conditions, along with two different background luminance conditions at the time of perceptual discrimination (i.e., at the time of probe flash occurrence). We describe these conditions in detail next. Each condition was tested separately in two sessions per subject. 
(1) Real saccades across a uniform background: Subjects started by fixating a white fixation spot that appeared at 11.2 degrees to the right or left of, and 3.8 degrees above or below, display center (Figure 1A, top). The spot was a square of 7.3 by 7.3 min arc size and 142.8 cd/m2 luminance (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). After a random delay of 800–1700 ms, the fixation spot jumped to display center, instructing a saccade towards the new spot location (Figure 1A, top). We then initiated an automatic program to detect saccade onset. This program was described previously (Chen & Hafed, 2013). Briefly, the program collected a recent history of eye position samples and used those to obtain a running estimate of eye speed. If such speed exceeded a threshold, a saccade was detected. Upon saccade detection, we presented a single-frame probe flash as the perceptual discrimination stimulus (Figure 1A', top). The probe flash occurred after one of four possible time points after online saccade detection: 24, 35, 47, or 59 ms. These time points were guided by our experience in a similar context (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020), and they allowed us to obtain a time course of recovery in perceptual sensitivity after the saccades. 
Figure 1.
 
The different saccade conditions tested in our study. (A) Subjects made approximately 11.8 degrees saccades towards display center from one of four possible locations. The saccades (schematized as curved arrows) were predominantly horizontal (see text), and we detected their onset online using a velocity criterion. Upon saccade detection, we presented a brief luminance flash at 7 degrees either above or below the saccade target (right column of images). The flash was presented at one of four different delays from online saccade detection (see B–D). In condition 1 (top row), the saccades were made over a uniform background. In condition 2 (middle row), gaze crossed a vertical luminance stripe. And, in condition 3 (bottom row), the saccades brought gaze across a vertical luminance edge. We varied background luminance or the combination of pre- and post-saccadic background luminances across trials, and the probe flashes could also be of either positive (luminance increments relative to the background) or negative (luminance decrements) polarity. (B) After detecting saccades offline, we plotted the distributions of flash times collected during the experiments in condition 1 across all subjects. Our experimental manipulation succeeded in probing multiple times of perceptual sensitivity after saccade onset. The jitter in the individual distributions was due to variability in online saccade detection, as well as display update timing variability (due to the asynchrony between saccades and display refresh clocks). The solid vertical lines show the means of each distribution. (C, D) Similar distributions of flash times for conditions 2 and 3, suggesting that differences in perceptual thresholds across conditions (see Results) were not attributable to different flash times relative to saccade onset. The vertical lines (dashed in C and dotted in D) show the average flash times for each colored distribution in the respective panel, and they are extended upward towards the distributions in B to facilitate comparison of mean flash times. The mean times (of each colored distribution) were very similar across conditions (with a maximal observed difference of 0.8 ms).
Figure 1.
 
The different saccade conditions tested in our study. (A) Subjects made approximately 11.8 degrees saccades towards display center from one of four possible locations. The saccades (schematized as curved arrows) were predominantly horizontal (see text), and we detected their onset online using a velocity criterion. Upon saccade detection, we presented a brief luminance flash at 7 degrees either above or below the saccade target (right column of images). The flash was presented at one of four different delays from online saccade detection (see B–D). In condition 1 (top row), the saccades were made over a uniform background. In condition 2 (middle row), gaze crossed a vertical luminance stripe. And, in condition 3 (bottom row), the saccades brought gaze across a vertical luminance edge. We varied background luminance or the combination of pre- and post-saccadic background luminances across trials, and the probe flashes could also be of either positive (luminance increments relative to the background) or negative (luminance decrements) polarity. (B) After detecting saccades offline, we plotted the distributions of flash times collected during the experiments in condition 1 across all subjects. Our experimental manipulation succeeded in probing multiple times of perceptual sensitivity after saccade onset. The jitter in the individual distributions was due to variability in online saccade detection, as well as display update timing variability (due to the asynchrony between saccades and display refresh clocks). The solid vertical lines show the means of each distribution. (C, D) Similar distributions of flash times for conditions 2 and 3, suggesting that differences in perceptual thresholds across conditions (see Results) were not attributable to different flash times relative to saccade onset. The vertical lines (dashed in C and dotted in D) show the average flash times for each colored distribution in the respective panel, and they are extended upward towards the distributions in B to facilitate comparison of mean flash times. The mean times (of each colored distribution) were very similar across conditions (with a maximal observed difference of 0.8 ms).
In all experimental conditions in which we used online saccade detection, we re-detected saccades offline, which allowed refining saccade onset time (Chen & Hafed, 2013); see the data analysis section below for further details. Across all conditions with saccades being detected online, we confirmed that the four probe flash times designated above were occurring at 39.53 ± 0.81, 51.38 ± 0.85, 63.05 ± 0.97, or 74.83 ± 0.85 ms (mean value ± SEM across all trials from all subjects) after saccade onset (Figures 1B–D shows the distributions of flash times in this and the two other saccade conditions described below). This was similar to the approach that we recently used (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). Moreover, we confirmed that the flash times after saccade onset were similar across all conditions employing real saccades (see the raw distributions in Figures 1B–D; there was a maximum of 0.8 ms difference in each same-colored distributions’ mean times across the three shown experimental conditions; compare vertical solid and dashed or dotted lines across conditions). This way, we could safely conclude that differences in the strength of perceptual saccadic suppression across the different conditions (see Results) could not be attributed to systematic differences in the flash times after saccades. 
It should also be noted that with online saccade detection, we did not present flashes pre-saccadically. This was a conscious choice given the large numbers of trials needed in all of our experiments to collect full psychometric curves at each flash time and for each background luminance and for each of either saccade or fixation conditions. This is a strategy that we had also used in our recent experiments (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). However, in other conditions described below (conditions 4 and 5), we did present flashes before visual transients, confirming that “pre-saccadic” suppression does occur, and that it is consistent with all of our previously described results motivating the current study (J. Bellet et al., 2017; Chen & Hafed, 2017; Hafed & Krauzlis, 2010; Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). 
The probe flash presented after saccade detection was a square of 2.4 degrees by 2.4 degrees dimensions (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). Its luminance consisted of either a decrement (shown in the examples of Figure 1A) or increment relative to the background luminance, allowing us to explore the impact of stimulus polarity on peri-saccadic perceptual sensitivity. The background luminance itself was either bright (77.3 cd/m2) or dark (22.4 cd/m2). 
Across trials, the probe flash could appear 7 degrees above or below the saccade target location. The subjects’ task was to indicate, by pressing one of two buttons on a response box, whether the flash appeared above or below screen center. Note that with our display geometry, the retinotopic position at which the probe flash could appear was visually stimulated before the saccade by the display itself (i.e., a uniform luminance) rather than the (very) dark surround of the laboratory. Thus, across the saccade, the possible retinotopic location of the flash was swept across a uniform luminance rather than across the outer edge of the display. 
We collected psychometric curves of flash perception as follows. Across trials, and for a given condition (for example: the combination of dark background; negative contrast flash; flash time 24 ms after saccade detection), we used an adaptive approach to select flash luminance levels resulting in threshold performance (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). Briefly, in the first session of this condition, we used a QUEST procedure (Watson & Pelli, 1983) for each flash polarity, each background luminance, and each post-saccadic flash time aiming to achieve a percent correct of 75% over 60 trials. Then, after the threshold contrast was found for each QUEST procedure, in the second session, we introduced, for each condition, six additional contrast levels around the detected perceptual threshold obtained with the adaptive procedure. This gave more samples for the psychometric curve of each subject (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). 
Note that in this and all other conditions and analyses throughout this study, we refer to “threshold” as the absolute value of the probe contrast that was used (whether the probe was a luminance increment or decrement). However, we always also explicitly specify whether the probe was of positive (luminance increment) or negative (luminance decrement) polarity. This way, it is simpler to quantitatively compare saccade-related threshold elevations for both types of probe flashes. 
We collected 1920 trials per subject in this condition. 
(2) Real saccades across a vertical visual stripe: This condition was identical to condition 1 except that each saccade crossed a vertical luminance stripe of width 2.4 degrees. The stripe was centered horizontally on the midway point between initial and final fixation spot locations, and it spanned the full vertical extent of the display (Figure 1A, middle). Therefore, on every trial, the display was configured to have two vertical luminance stripes at 5.7 degrees either to the right or left of display center. The left stripe was crossed by rightward saccades starting from the left side of the display, and the right stripe was crossed by leftward saccades. Across trials, we varied the luminance of either the background or the stripes. Specifically, the stripes had the dark luminance when the rest of the display had the bright luminance, and vice versa (same luminance values as in condition 1). This way, the eye could start and land on either a dark or bright luminance region (like in condition 1), but the difference is that the gaze would have had to cross a luminance stripe of the other luminosity during the saccade. Like in condition 1, the retinal positions of the probe flash locations were visually stimulated by the display before the saccade (rather than by the much darker laboratory surround). 
We collected 1920 trials per subject from this condition. 
(3) Real saccades across a vertical luminance edge: In this condition, the display was split (along the horizontal dimension) into three areas (one central and two flanks; Figure 1A, bottom): the central region had either a bright or dark luminance, and the flanks were the opposite. The vertical edge between the central region and either of the two flanking regions was at 5.7 degrees horizontally from display center, which is halfway between the initial and final fixation spot's horizontal locations. As a result, there was a vertical edge across which the same saccades as in conditions 1 and 2 were made. The bright and dark luminances on either side of the vertical edge were the same as those of the bright and dark background luminances in conditions 1 and 2, and they were varied across trials (some trials had the central area being bright, and others had the flanking areas being bright, independently of whether the saccade was rightward or leftward). Thus, in this condition, the saccade landed on either a bright or a dark luminance level (like in conditions 1 and 2); however, the initial luminance level at saccade onset was always different from the final luminance level (Figure 1A, bottom). Moreover, the eye crossed only a single vertical luminance edge during the saccade, as opposed to two edges in condition 2. Finally, the probe flashes (whether luminance increments or luminance decrements) were always presented relative to the luminance level at the end of the saccade. That is, the probe flashes appeared on either bright or dark backgrounds (like in conditions 1 and 2). 
We collected 1920 trials per subject from this condition. 
(4) Simulated saccades across a vertical luminance edge: Subjects maintained fixation at display center, and a visual transient like that in condition 3 was introduced. That is, the display had a central region that was either bright or dark and two flanking regions around it with the opposite brightness from the central region (Figure 2A). Thus, there was a vertical edge, which was then translated horizontally to simulate a saccade across this edge. To achieve the translation, we moved the whole displayed texture (i.e., both the central and flanking regions) horizontally at high speed. The resulting “simulated” saccade had parameters similar to those used in our recent work (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). Briefly, we translated the texture (and, therefore, the vertical edge) by 1.9 degrees every display frame (of approximately 12 ms) for six display frames, corresponding to an overall translation of 11.4 degrees in 72 ms. This resulted in a high-speed translation of the vertical edge across the fovea during maintained fixation. The translation matched, in amplitude and duration, the amplitudes and durations of the real saccades that the subjects made in conditions 1–3 above. Specifically, in these conditions, the average saccade amplitude and duration were 11.37 ± 0.14 degrees and 74.89 ± 2.05 ms, respectively, across all trials from all subjects (mean value ± SEM). 
Figure 2.
 
The visual-only conditions tested in our study. (A) In condition 4, subjects fixated the center of the display, and the background was identical to that of condition 3 in Figure 1. After initial fixation was established, the whole background translated horizontally to shift one of the two vertical edges across gaze position. After the translation, gaze was now aimed at a different background luminance. This condition mimicked the real saccade version in condition 3. Note that during translation, we treated the texture as being horizontally periodic (in the shown example, this meant that a bright background was revealed on the right edge of the display monitor during leftward translation). At different times relative to translation onset, we presented a flash either above or below fixation, similar to Figure 1A. In this case, the flashes could be presented either before or after translation onset (Methods). When presented after translation onset, the timing was such that they were presented over the new background luminance after translation end (that is, the edge had crossed display center even for the earliest flash time after translation onset). (B) In condition 5, the subjects fixated the center of a uniform display, and we changed the luminance of the background (from dark to bright or vice versa). Relative to this transient, a probe flash could happen at similar times to those used in A. Note that in this condition only, we had four possible probe flash locations, because this experiment was a replication of our earlier version of it in (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020; Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020).
Figure 2.
 
The visual-only conditions tested in our study. (A) In condition 4, subjects fixated the center of the display, and the background was identical to that of condition 3 in Figure 1. After initial fixation was established, the whole background translated horizontally to shift one of the two vertical edges across gaze position. After the translation, gaze was now aimed at a different background luminance. This condition mimicked the real saccade version in condition 3. Note that during translation, we treated the texture as being horizontally periodic (in the shown example, this meant that a bright background was revealed on the right edge of the display monitor during leftward translation). At different times relative to translation onset, we presented a flash either above or below fixation, similar to Figure 1A. In this case, the flashes could be presented either before or after translation onset (Methods). When presented after translation onset, the timing was such that they were presented over the new background luminance after translation end (that is, the edge had crossed display center even for the earliest flash time after translation onset). (B) In condition 5, the subjects fixated the center of a uniform display, and we changed the luminance of the background (from dark to bright or vice versa). Relative to this transient, a probe flash could happen at similar times to those used in A. Note that in this condition only, we had four possible probe flash locations, because this experiment was a replication of our earlier version of it in (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020; Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020).
The sequence of events in a given trial was as follows. An initial fixation spot appeared at the center of the display. The two vertical edges were at 5.7 degrees to the left and right of the fixation spot location (Figure 2A). After a random time of 800–1700 ms, the high-speed translation was started. For a leftward translation, the right vertical edge shifted to become now located at the initial location of the left vertical edge after the entire sequence had ended, and vice versa for a rightward texture shift. To handle display monitor edge effects, we treated the initial displayed pattern (i.e., the whole of the combination of central region and two flanks) as a horizontally periodic pattern. As a result, when a leftward shift happened, the rightward flank translated to become the central region, and a new rightward flank that was the same luminance as the original central region appeared. The corresponding scenario played out for a rightward shift. A probe flash appeared similar to the other conditions described above. 
Across trials, we picked two times of probe flashes after the onset of the simulated saccades (47 and 59 ms) and also two times before the onset of the simulated saccades (24 and 35 ms before simulated saccade onset). The use of probe flashes before simulated saccade onset allowed us to demonstrate existence of pretransient suppression, which provides a logical link to our other work (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020), and which also demonstrates that our lack of pre-saccadic flash times in conditions 1 to 3 above does not necessarily mean that there was no pre-saccadic suppression in these conditions. All other parameters of the experiment were similar to conditions 1 to 3. 
We collected 1920 trials per subject from this condition. 
(5) Fixation with a luminance change: Subjects maintained fixation at the center of a uniform display, and the visual transient now consisted of a change in background luminance from dark to bright, or vice versa (Figure 2B). Probe flashes could appear either before (24 or 35 ms) or after (35, 71, or 106 ms) the luminance change, like in condition 4. Note that in this condition only, we used a four-alternative forced choice paradigm as opposed to the two-alternative forced choice paradigm in all other conditions. So, instead of just presenting a probe flash 7 degrees above or below the fixation (or saccade) target location, the probe flash could now appear at 7 degrees in any of the four cardinal directions around the fixation spot (right, left, up, or down; Figure 2B). The reason for this is that fixation was always at the center of the display and that the display itself was uniform (allowing us to probe horizontal flash locations without worrying about “pre-saccadic” retinal image regions of horizontal flash locations being visually stimulated by the dark laboratory surround instead of the actual display, or by another luminance on the display like in condition 4). This condition also involved collecting new data from a similar condition (with four alternatives) that we had recently run (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020; Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020). All other parameters and procedures were the same as in all other conditions. 
We collected 2400 trials per subject from this condition. 
Data analysis
We detected saccades and microsaccades using our laboratory's established methods (M. E. Bellet, Bellet, Nienborg, Hafed, & Berens, 2019; Chen & Hafed, 2013; Hafed, 2013). For all trials with fixation (conditions 4 and 5), we ensured that there were no microsaccades from –200 to +50 ms relative to probe flash onset. This allowed us to avoid potential changes in visual sensitivity around the time of microsaccades (Chen & Hafed, 2017; Chen et al., 2015; Hafed, Chen, & Tian, 2015). Similarly, for trials with real saccades, we excluded trials with saccades before instruction (i.e., before fixation spot jump) and also trials with saccadic reaction times >500 ms from saccade target onset, and we confirmed that the flash times relative to saccade onset (after post-hoc offline saccade detection) indeed clustered into four different time points, as designed in the task (see above and Figures 1B–D). 
We analyzed the proportion of correct trials as a function of probe flash Weber contrast (i.e., absolute value of luminance difference of flash from background luminance, divided by the background luminance). We used Weber contrast, as opposed to Michelson contrast, because our probe flashes were relatively small relative to the uniform background across which they were presented; therefore, the average luminance with the flash was similar to the background luminance, making Weber contrast a more suitable metric. We calculated the proportion of correct trials for each flash luminance used, and independently for whether the probe flash consisted of a luminance increment or a luminance decrement relative to background luminance. We also did this for each flash time used. 
For real saccades, we classified all flash times into four “clusters” of flash times centered around the mean values measured after offline saccade detection (in other words, each individually colored distribution of trials in Figures 1B–D contributed to one flash time “cluster” in the analyses). The results with real saccades were not altered when we first combined all flash times in a given condition, regardless of the online-designated delays in Figures 1B–D, and re-binned the trials as a function of time from saccade onset; our choice to use the colored clusters in Figures 1B–D for the temporal binning was only done for simplicity. 
Across contrasts (for either increments or decrements), we obtained a psychometric curve fit of perceptual performance using the psignifit 4 toolbox (Schutt, Harmeling, Macke, & Wichmann, 2016). Briefly, we used a beta-binomial model for the psychometric curve. We defined the perceptual threshold as the absolute Weber contrast value resulting in a correct performance level that was at either 75% of the total dynamic range of the fitted psychometric curve (for conditions 1–4 with two-alternative forced choices) or 62.5% of the total dynamic range of the psychometric curve (for condition 5 with four-alternative forced choices). If there was perceptual suppression of sensitivity, then the threshold contrast was elevated. Thus, plots of threshold contrast indicate maximal suppression when the perceptual thresholds are high. 
For each background luminance (dark or bright), and for each flash time, we calculated a psychometric curve for either flashes consisting of luminance increments or luminance decrements (i.e., flash stimulus polarity). We used the longer flash times after visual transient onset (either caused by real saccades or visual display updates during fixation) to confirm that there was perceptual recovery with time. Therefore, elevations of threshold contrast above the threshold values at such longer post-transient times were evidence for perceptual suppression. 
To summarize results, we first estimated perceptual thresholds for each individual subject, and we then averaged the thresholds across subjects, with indications of inter-subject variance in all figures. To perform statistics, we used repeated-measures two-way ANOVA tests comparing the influences of flash time and condition on perceptual thresholds. We also sometimes performed individual t-tests comparing pairs of conditions at one given flash time; typically, this was the shortest flash time after saccade onset (or after texture translation or contrast change in conditions 4 and 5) since this time was the time associated with maximal perceptual suppression. This flash time was therefore of most interest when comparing suppression strength across the different conditions. 
It should also be noted that in conditions 4 and 5, when we had “pre-transient” flashes, the flash appeared on a background that was different from the background for flash onsets occurring after the visual transient. Therefore, in all figures, we always pooled data points based on the background luminance on which a flash actually appeared. For example, pre-transient flashes in condition 4 with, say, a translation from bright to dark background were plotted as flashes over a bright background, whereas post-transient flashes were plotted as flashes over a dark background. This way, we always compared perception with a similar relationship between flash luminance and background luminance at the time of the flash. In statistical analyses of condition 5, we also pooled together, within each subject, the longest two flash times after background luminance change. This was because these two times showed the same effects anyway. 
Results
We assessed the visual contributions to perceptual saccadic suppression by measuring perceptual sensitivity for brief flashes presented around the time of saccades across a variety of image types. We also compared such sensitivity to situations in which there were no saccades, but in which comparable visual transients occurred on the retina. In all cases, we compared perceptual sensitivity to a baseline case in which saccades were made across a uniform background. The results that we present below confirm and extend our recent observations that perceptual saccadic suppression may fundamentally be a visual phenomenon (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020; Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020), and they are also in line with other psychophysical evidence in the literature. 
Saccades across a local luminance feature are associated with much stronger perceptual suppression than saccades across a uniform background
We collected perceptual threshold measurements across saccades. In one condition (condition 1), subjects made predominantly horizontal oblique saccades across a uniform background. In another condition, the saccades were made across a 2.4 degrees wide luminance stripe relative to background luminance, but the luminances at both saccade start and saccade end were the same (condition 2). Figure 3 shows example psychometric curves of perceptual detectability obtained from an example subject in the two conditions and at flash times of approximately 40 ms (Figure 3A) and 63 ms (Figure 3B) after saccade onset. In this figure, we show results only from the dark background condition (Methods), and also when the luminance of the probe flash was a decrement relative to background luminance (i.e., negative stimulus polarity). Note that in this and all other figures, we depict the absolute value of Weber contrast, to aid comparison of effects between positive-contrast and negative-contrast probe flashes. Flashes immediately after saccade detection (Figure 3A) were associated with elevated perceptual thresholds in both conditions: this is evidenced by rightward shifts of the psychometric curves for the early time compared to the late one (compare the correspondingly colored curves in Figure 3A and Figure 3B). However, the effect was much stronger for condition 2, in which saccades crossed over a bright luminance stripe of 2.4 degrees width. Specifically, at 40 ms (Figure 3A), the absolute value of perceptual threshold was at >0.8 Weber contrast in condition 2 and at only <0.15 Weber contrast in condition 1. Similarly, at the later time point, even after partial perceptual recovery (Figure 3B), the absolute value of threshold in condition 2 was still at a level of almost 0.3 Weber contrast, but it was already at a level <0.09 in condition 1. Thus, perceptual saccadic suppression is much stronger when saccades are made across a luminance stripe than across a uniform background. 
Figure 3.
 
Much stronger saccadic suppression across a luminance stripe than across a uniform background. Example psychometric curves of perceptual detectability from one sample subject, for dark probe flashes over a dark background. Circles show individual data points, with the size of the circle scaled based on the numbers of observations collected. The blue curves show psychometric curves when the saccade crossed a luminance stripe (condition 2), whereas the black curves show the results obtained when the saccade was made across a uniform background (condition 1); see the schematic legend in B. Vertical lines indicate the perceptual threshold values in each condition, and horizontal error bars show 95% confidence intervals for each psychometric curve (obtained using the psignifit 4 toolbox; Methods). (A) Data for probe flashes presented immediately after online saccade detection (40 ms after actual saccade onset; first time cluster in Figures 1B–D). There was a much larger rightward shift in the psychometric curve in the case of condition 2 when compared to condition 1. (B) Data for a later flash time (63 ms after saccade onset; third time cluster in Figures 1B–D). Both curves recovered at this time point (i.e., shifted leftward relative to A; horizontal leftward arrows), but the blue curve still showed much more suppression (i.e., rightward shift) compared to the black curve.
Figure 3.
 
Much stronger saccadic suppression across a luminance stripe than across a uniform background. Example psychometric curves of perceptual detectability from one sample subject, for dark probe flashes over a dark background. Circles show individual data points, with the size of the circle scaled based on the numbers of observations collected. The blue curves show psychometric curves when the saccade crossed a luminance stripe (condition 2), whereas the black curves show the results obtained when the saccade was made across a uniform background (condition 1); see the schematic legend in B. Vertical lines indicate the perceptual threshold values in each condition, and horizontal error bars show 95% confidence intervals for each psychometric curve (obtained using the psignifit 4 toolbox; Methods). (A) Data for probe flashes presented immediately after online saccade detection (40 ms after actual saccade onset; first time cluster in Figures 1B–D). There was a much larger rightward shift in the psychometric curve in the case of condition 2 when compared to condition 1. (B) Data for a later flash time (63 ms after saccade onset; third time cluster in Figures 1B–D). Both curves recovered at this time point (i.e., shifted leftward relative to A; horizontal leftward arrows), but the blue curve still showed much more suppression (i.e., rightward shift) compared to the black curve.
Across subjects, we made very similar observations. For example, the blue curve in Figure 4A shows the average psychometric curve obtained across all subjects (the average of all subjects’ individual psychometric curves) in condition 2 for the first flash time (40 ms after saccade onset). This curve was shifted much more strongly to the right than the black curve in the same panel. The black curve summarizes the results across subjects with only a uniform background (condition 1). Similarly, Figure 4B shows recovery results at the 63 ms time point in the same format, again consistent with Figure 3. To summarize the effect size and time course to recovery across the two conditions, we then estimated the perceptual threshold for a given condition as the absolute value of Weber contrast in each psychometric curve resulting in a correct performance level of 75% of the dynamic range (Methods, compare the vertical lines in Figure 3) (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). Figure 4C plots the absolute threshold contrasts at all probe flash times (Figure 1B, C): in blue for condition 2 and in black for condition 1. Error bars denote SEM across subjects (Methods). There was much stronger suppression (larger increase in the perceptual threshold contrast) for saccades across the luminance stripe (blue) when compared to saccades across the uniform background (black), and this also persisted as a function of time. Statistically, a repeated-measures two-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect of both factors of flash time (F(3,3) = 61.4, p < 0.01) and condition (F(1,5) = 96, p < 0.001). Moreover, there was also a significant interaction between the two factors (F(3,3) = 31.1, p < 0.01). Post hoc, we compared the thresholds of condition 1 (mean ± SEM: 0.13 ± 0.02 Weber contrast) and condition 2 (0.95 ± 0.1 Weber contrast) at 40 ms (the time of maximal suppression in our experiments). There was a significant difference around the time of maximal suppression (t(5) = 7.9, p < 0.001). 
Figure 4.
 
Systematically stronger saccadic suppression across a luminance stripe or luminance edge than across a uniform background. (A) Average psychometric curves of perceptual detectability from all subjects when the probe flashes occurred 40 ms after saccade onset (first time cluster in Figures 1B–D). Black: psychometric curve for condition 1, when saccades were made across a uniform background. Blue: psychometric curve when saccades crossed a luminance stripe (condition 2). Red: psychometric curve for condition 3, when saccades crossed a luminance edge. In all cases, error bands denote SEM across subjects. Perceptual thresholds were elevated much more for conditions 2 and 3 than for condition 1. (B) Same as in A but for a later time point (63 ms). There was perceptual recovery, but saccadic suppression was still stronger in conditions 2 and 3 than in condition 1; the subjects always had higher thresholds than in condition 1 (also see C). (C) Perceptual thresholds (average across subjects) for each flash time after saccade onset. Higher values mean stronger saccadic suppression. Error bars denote SEM across subjects. Note that the time values on the x-axis are the average flash times obtained after offline saccade detection (Methods and Figures 1B–D). Also note that for each average flash time, we slightly jittered the x-axis position of each data point to not mask individual data points and their error bars (also true in subsequent figures below). There was much stronger saccadic suppression in conditions 2 and 3 than in condition 1.
Figure 4.
 
Systematically stronger saccadic suppression across a luminance stripe or luminance edge than across a uniform background. (A) Average psychometric curves of perceptual detectability from all subjects when the probe flashes occurred 40 ms after saccade onset (first time cluster in Figures 1B–D). Black: psychometric curve for condition 1, when saccades were made across a uniform background. Blue: psychometric curve when saccades crossed a luminance stripe (condition 2). Red: psychometric curve for condition 3, when saccades crossed a luminance edge. In all cases, error bands denote SEM across subjects. Perceptual thresholds were elevated much more for conditions 2 and 3 than for condition 1. (B) Same as in A but for a later time point (63 ms). There was perceptual recovery, but saccadic suppression was still stronger in conditions 2 and 3 than in condition 1; the subjects always had higher thresholds than in condition 1 (also see C). (C) Perceptual thresholds (average across subjects) for each flash time after saccade onset. Higher values mean stronger saccadic suppression. Error bars denote SEM across subjects. Note that the time values on the x-axis are the average flash times obtained after offline saccade detection (Methods and Figures 1B–D). Also note that for each average flash time, we slightly jittered the x-axis position of each data point to not mask individual data points and their error bars (also true in subsequent figures below). There was much stronger saccadic suppression in conditions 2 and 3 than in condition 1.
Different pre- and post-saccadic visual stimulation is also associated with strong perceptual saccadic suppression
We next asked whether the final luminance on which the probe flashes appeared peri-saccadically needed to be the same as the “pre-saccadic” luminance at the line of sight for us to obtain the above results. To test this, we replaced the stripe used in condition 2 with a luminance edge (condition 3; Methods; red curves in Figure 4). That is, this time, the subjects initially fixated a bright background, and they then made saccades across a vertical edge, such that the probe flashes (and saccade landing positions) were now over a dark background. The same strong difference in saccadic suppression relative to the uniform dark background was observed (Figure 4, now compare red to black curves). For example, at the first flash time after saccade onset (Figures 4A, C), the average perceptual threshold in both conditions 2 and 3 was >0.75 Weber contrast, whereas it was only 0.13 Weber contrast when saccades were made across a uniform background (Figure 4C). Moreover, statistically, there was still a significant effect of time (F(3,3) = 13.6, p < 0.05) and condition (F(1,5) = 41.5, p < 0.01), when now comparing condition 3 to condition 1 (repeated-measures two-way ANOVA). There was also a significant interaction effect (F(3,3) = 9.9, p < 0.05). Moreover, a paired post hoc t-test highlighted a difference at 40 ms (t(5) = 6.72, p = 0.001; condition 1: 0.13 ± 0.02 SEM Weber contrast, and condition 3: 0.76 ± 0.11 SEM Weber contrast). In fact, when we compared threshold contrasts across subjects in both conditions 2 and 3 together (colored curves in Figure 4C), we found that suppression was equally strong for both conditions. A repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with the factors time and condition (2 versus 3) revealed no significant interaction between the factors (F(3,3) = 2.4, p = 0.24) and no significant effect of the condition (F(1,5) = 1.3, p = 0.3). However, expectedly, the effect of flash time was significant (F(3,3) = 51.5, p < 0.01). 
Therefore, saccadic suppression is also enhanced when gaze crosses a luminance edge rather than a luminance stripe, replicating results from an earlier study (Maij, Matziridi, Smeets, & Brenner, 2012). The difference here is that in our current experiments, we assessed thresholds explicitly, by collecting full psychometric curves. We also varied stimulus polarities of the flashes, and even replaced saccades with image translations, as we describe in more detail shortly. 
It is important to note that saccadic suppression still occurred over a uniform background (condition 1), albeit in a weak fashion. To confirm this, we statistically assessed perceptual detectability in condition 1 with a dark background, and we found that there was elevation of contrast immediately after saccade detection when compared to the latest two flash times after saccade onset in Figure 4C (black): a repeated-measures one-way ANOVA with flash time as factor was significant (F(3,3) = 22.5, p = 0.015), and a post hoc test revealed significant differences for the perceptual thresholds at 40 and 63 ms (p = 0.035), 40 and 75 ms (p = 0.009), and 51 and 75 ms (p = 0.002). Similar results also occurred for a bright background (we address the influence of background luminance more explicitly shortly). This extends the results of (Maij et al., 2012) in an important way. In that study, it could appear that there was no saccadic suppression at all with a uniform background. However, we think that measuring contrast thresholds in our experiments was a more sensitive measure of perceptual sensitivity. The approach of Maij et al could result in floor or ceiling effects, potentially masking a mild amount of suppression (which we observed). 
A visual dependence of saccadic suppression also emerges when considering the luminance of the background across which saccades are made
The above results indicate that the visual conditions across which saccades are made do matter for perceptual saccadic suppression, as we also recently argued (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). To further demonstrate that perceptual saccadic suppression does indeed have a strong visual component, we next checked whether the luminance of the background mattered for the strength of suppression (Brooks, Impelman, & Lum, 1980). In our experiments, saccades could be made in condition 1 across either a bright or a dark background (Methods). Similarly, in conditions 2 and 3, the final landing position of the saccades, and therefore the final possible probe flash locations, could be either on a bright background or a dark background. In the description of results so far we have focused only on the dark background condition. We now describe the threshold contrasts that were obtained when the probe flashes appeared on a bright background instead. 
Figures 5A, B show average psychometric curves across subjects for the first flash time after saccade detection (approximately 40 ms from saccade onset). Here, we directly compare trials having a dark background at flash occurrence (solid lines, replicated from Figure 4A) with trials having a bright background at flash occurrence (dashed lines). In Figure 5A, the data for condition 2 are shown, and in Figure 5B, the data for condition 3 are shown. In both cases, saccadic suppression was much stronger over a dark landing position than over a bright landing position. This effect can be better seen through plots of the time course of saccadic suppression in the two conditions (Figure 5C). Statistically comparing bright versus dark backgrounds, as well as time, in repeated-measures two-way ANOVA tests confirmed that there was a strong effect of background luminance (and time) on saccadic suppression. For both conditions, a repeated-measures two-way ANOVA revealed significant interactions (condition 2: F(3,3) = 73.9, p < 0.01; condition 3: F(3,3) = 12.5, p < 0.05) between the factors, as well as significant main effects for time (condition 2: F(3,3) = 41.5, p < 0.01; condition 3: F(3,3) = 10.8, p < 0.05) and background luminance (condition 2: F(1,5) = 86.7, p < 0.001; condition 3: F(1,5) = 16.4, p < 0.01). Note that for simplicity, we still only considered flashes consisting of luminance decrements relative to the background (i.e., of negative stimulus polarity), exactly like in all analyses above. We will explicitly describe the impact of flash luminance polarity relative to the background in a subsequent analysis. In any case, threshold elevations were much stronger with dark backgrounds than with bright backgrounds. 
Figure 5.
 
Stronger saccadic suppression over dark backgrounds, whether with luminance edges or luminance stripes. (A) Average psychometric curves of perceptual detectability from all subjects in condition 2, and for probes occurring 40 ms after saccade onset (i.e., the first time cluster in Figures 1B–D). Solid indicates flashes over a dark background (i.e., saccades starting and ending on a dark patch, but crossing a bright stripe), and dashed indicates flashes over a bright background (i.e., saccades starting and ending on a bright patch, but crossing a dark stripe). In both cases, the probe flashes had negative luminance polarity (i.e., they were luminance decrements relative to the background). Saccadic suppression was stronger with dark backgrounds than with bright backgrounds. Error bands denote SEM across subjects. (B) Similar results were obtained in condition 3. Flashes over a dark background (when saccades were made from a bright to a dark patch) were associated with stronger suppression than flashes over a bright background (when saccades were made from a dark to a bright patch). (C) Perceptual thresholds (averaged across subjects) for each flash time after saccade onset. Higher values mean stronger saccadic suppression, and error bars denote SEM across the subjects. In both conditions 2 and 3, flashes over a dark background (solid) were associated with much stronger saccadic suppression than flashes over a bright background (dashed). All other conventions are similar to Figure 4.
Figure 5.
 
Stronger saccadic suppression over dark backgrounds, whether with luminance edges or luminance stripes. (A) Average psychometric curves of perceptual detectability from all subjects in condition 2, and for probes occurring 40 ms after saccade onset (i.e., the first time cluster in Figures 1B–D). Solid indicates flashes over a dark background (i.e., saccades starting and ending on a dark patch, but crossing a bright stripe), and dashed indicates flashes over a bright background (i.e., saccades starting and ending on a bright patch, but crossing a dark stripe). In both cases, the probe flashes had negative luminance polarity (i.e., they were luminance decrements relative to the background). Saccadic suppression was stronger with dark backgrounds than with bright backgrounds. Error bands denote SEM across subjects. (B) Similar results were obtained in condition 3. Flashes over a dark background (when saccades were made from a bright to a dark patch) were associated with stronger suppression than flashes over a bright background (when saccades were made from a dark to a bright patch). (C) Perceptual thresholds (averaged across subjects) for each flash time after saccade onset. Higher values mean stronger saccadic suppression, and error bars denote SEM across the subjects. In both conditions 2 and 3, flashes over a dark background (solid) were associated with much stronger saccadic suppression than flashes over a bright background (dashed). All other conventions are similar to Figure 4.
Incidentally, for saccades across a uniform background (condition 1), a repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with the factors time and background luminance showed no interaction between the factors (F(3,3) = 0.1, p = 0.95). As expected, since suppression still took place in this condition, the main effects of time and background were still significant (time: F(3,3) = 19.5, p < 0.05; background luminance: F(1,5) = 9.1, p < 0.05; also see Figure 6). 
Figure 6.
 
Stronger saccadic suppression for negative polarity flashes over dark backgrounds when compared to positive polarity flashes. Perceptual thresholds across subjects as a function of flash time after saccade onset (error bars denote SEM across subjects). (A) Data from condition 2 (blue) and condition 1 (black) with flashes appearing over a dark background. The saturated colors show results with negative polarity flashes, and the unsaturated colors show results with positive polarity flashes. Consistent with earlier figures, perceptual saccadic suppression was much stronger (higher absolute values of perceptual thresholds) in condition 2 than in condition 1 (compare colored to black curves). Within each condition, saccadic suppression was stronger with negative polarity flashes than positive polarity flashes, especially near the time of peak saccadic suppression (40 ms). (B) With bright backgrounds, the effects of flash polarity were absent (but suppression was still stronger than in condition 1). Note that the y-axis is different from A, because suppression was weaker for flashes over bright backgrounds (see Figure 5). (C, D) Similar results from condition 3, with saccades crossing a luminance edge. The black curves in this case are the same as in A, B. All other conventions are similar to Figures 45.
Figure 6.
 
Stronger saccadic suppression for negative polarity flashes over dark backgrounds when compared to positive polarity flashes. Perceptual thresholds across subjects as a function of flash time after saccade onset (error bars denote SEM across subjects). (A) Data from condition 2 (blue) and condition 1 (black) with flashes appearing over a dark background. The saturated colors show results with negative polarity flashes, and the unsaturated colors show results with positive polarity flashes. Consistent with earlier figures, perceptual saccadic suppression was much stronger (higher absolute values of perceptual thresholds) in condition 2 than in condition 1 (compare colored to black curves). Within each condition, saccadic suppression was stronger with negative polarity flashes than positive polarity flashes, especially near the time of peak saccadic suppression (40 ms). (B) With bright backgrounds, the effects of flash polarity were absent (but suppression was still stronger than in condition 1). Note that the y-axis is different from A, because suppression was weaker for flashes over bright backgrounds (see Figure 5). (C, D) Similar results from condition 3, with saccades crossing a luminance edge. The black curves in this case are the same as in A, B. All other conventions are similar to Figures 45.
Therefore, the results of Figure 5 further illustrate the strong visual dependencies of perceptual saccadic suppression. When (negative polarity) probe flashes were presented over dark backgrounds, perceptual thresholds were elevated much more around the time of saccades than when the same negative polarity flashes (of the same contrast) were presented over bright backgrounds. Earlier work related to condition 3 did not analyze the effects of background luminance (Maij et al., 2012) (but see Brooks et al., 1980, which characterized the influence of background luminance for a subset of the conditions that we tested), and our recent experiments only used textured backgrounds (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). 
Flash luminance polarity interacts with background luminance to modulate the strength of saccadic suppression
We observed above that saccades across luminance-defined features are associated with strong perceptual suppression (Figures 34), and that background luminance interacts with the presented brief flashes to modulate the strength of suppression (Figure 5). However, in all of our analyses so far, we only considered negative polarity probe flashes. To check whether probe flash polarity constitutes an additional visual dependence of perceptual saccadic suppression, we then turned our attention to analyzing trials with positive polarity probe flashes. 
In Figure 6, we plotted the threshold contrasts across subjects for positive and negative polarity flashes in conditions 1, 2, and 3. In Figures 6A, C, we show the results with flashes presented over a dark background, and in Figures 6B, D, we show the results with flashes presented over a bright background. The top row of the figure (Figures 6A, B) shows results from condition 2 (blue), whereas the bottom row (Figures 6C, D) shows results from condition 3 (red). For each condition, the different shades of color denote whether the flash had negative polarity (saturated colors; these are data replicated from the previous figures) or positive polarity (unsaturated colors) relative to the background. Note that both rows also show the results from condition 1 with a uniform background for reference (black and gray curves). Also note that the y-axes in Figures 6A, C are different from those in Figures 6B, D because of the differential impact of background luminance that we described in Figure 5. As can be seen from Figures 6A, C, negative polarity probe flashes over dark backgrounds had stronger saccadic suppression (higher detection thresholds) than positive polarity probe flashes over the same background, especially at the time of peak saccadic suppression (40 ms). For example, immediately after saccade onset in conditions 2 and 3, perceptual thresholds were at 0.95 and 0.76 Weber contrast across subjects for negative polarity flashes, respectively (Figures 6A, C, colored curves). For positive polarity flashes, the thresholds were elevated to only 0.69 and 0.54, respectively. Statistically, this difference between positive and negative polarity flashes was significant at 40 ms in both condition 2 (t(5) = 6.69, p = 0.001; positive polarity mean and SEM: 0.95 ± 0.1 Weber contrast; negative polarity: 0.69 ± 0.07 Weber contrast) and condition 3 (paired t-test: t(5) = 3.31, p = 0.02; positive polarity mean and SEM: 0.76 ± 0.11 Weber contrast; negative polarity: 0.54 ± 0.07 Weber contrast). Interestingly, over a bright background (Figures 6B, D), both conditions 2 and 3 did not show any difference in the strength of saccadic suppression between positive and negative polarity flashes (Figures 6B, D, colored curves). Therefore, there was an interaction between flash polarity and background luminance in modulating the strength of perceptual saccadic suppression, which was also evident statistically. For example, at peak suppression, a repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with the factors background luminance and flash polarity revealed a significant interaction effect (condition 2: F(1,5) = 40.1, p < 0.01 and condition 3: F(1,5) = 13.8, p < 0.05). 
It is also interesting that for condition 1, similar effects still existed for flash luminance polarity (Figure 6, black and gray curves) despite the very weak perceptual saccadic suppression that occurred: it was still the case that negative polarity flashes over a dark background (mean threshold 0.13 ± 0.02 SEM Weber contrast) tended to cause stronger saccadic suppression than positive polarity flashes (mean threshold: 0.09 ± 0.01 SEM) over the same background (t(5) = 3.6, p = 0.016 Weber contrast). Thus, our analyses of probe flash stimulus polarity in all saccade conditions (1, 2, and 3) provide further evidence that perceptual saccadic suppression has strong visual dependencies. 
Perceptual suppression occurs equally well, and with the same visual dependencies, for image sweeps that conceptually mimic saccade-induced image shifts
The above evidence is indicative of strong and rich visual-visual interactions in saccadic suppression between the saccade-related retinal image shifts and the probe flashes themselves, as we have recently suggested (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020; Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020). If that is indeed the case, then the same visual dependencies as those demonstrated in Figures 36 should also happen in the complete absence of saccades, but in the presence of saccade-like image shifts and probe flashes. We therefore repeated the above experiments but with so-called “simulated saccades.” We asked subjects to fixate, and we swept a vertical luminance-defined edge across the retina to simulate a saccade-like image displacement (Figure 2A). This condition was therefore conceptually similar to condition 3 (Methods). The probe flashes happened identically to how they happened in the real saccade version of the experiment. We also additionally tested pre-shift probe flashes, to check for perceptual suppression before “simulated saccade” onset (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). 
We replicated all of the above observations that were made with real saccades. First, we confirmed that immediately after saccade-like image translation, strong perceptual suppression occurred, which was much stronger than the suppression with real saccades across a uniform background. This effect is demonstrated in Figure 7A, which shows the average psychometric curve across all subjects when a negative polarity flash appeared over a dark background at 47 ms after image translation onset (magenta curve). For reference, the black curve in the same figure shows the average psychometric curve associated with “real” saccadic suppression over a uniform (dark) background at our shortest flash time (i.e., associated with maximal saccadic suppression in our data set). As can be seen, perceptual thresholds were strongly elevated even with the simulated saccades, just like in condition 3. Across flash times, Figure 7B shows that immediately after image shift onset, the absolute value of threshold contrast over a dark background was 0.54 Weber contrast for negative polarity flashes. This value was much higher than that for real saccades across uniform backgrounds (0.13 Weber contrast for the shortest flash time with the real saccade; 40 ms). Statistically, we confirmed this by doing a paired comparison of thresholds between condition 4 (0.54 ± 0.11 SEM Weber contrast) and condition 1 (0.13 ± 0.02 SEM Weber contrast) at the shortest positive flash time after either image translation onset or saccade onset (t(5) = 4.7, p = 0.005). 
Figure 7.
 
Highly similar visual dependencies of perceptual suppression in the absence of saccades. (A) Psychometric curve of perceptual detectability from condition 4, in which a saccade-like displacement of a vertical luminance edge took place (conceptually similar to condition 3 with real saccades). For reference, the black curve shows the psychometric curve from condition 1 (real saccades over a uniform background) at 40 ms. The saccade-like image displacement caused large suppression (a larger rightward shift of the curve), similar to the large suppression seen with real saccades (e.g., Figure 4). Error bands denote SEM across subjects. (B) Time course of perceptual suppression in condition 4. For reference, the time course of saccadic suppression over a uniform background (condition 1) is also shown in black. (C) There was also stronger suppression with (negative polarity) flashes over a dark background than with the same flashes over a bright background, exactly like with real saccades (e.g., Figure 5). The average psychometric curve was shifted more to the right with a dark background (solid). (D) The dependence of perceptual suppression on background luminance was also clear in the whole time course, and this dependence was similar to that seen with real saccades in a similar visual condition of crossing an edge (e.g., Figure 5). For reference, the black curves show the results obtained with real saccades across a uniform dark (solid) or bright (dashed) background (same as in Figure 6).
Figure 7.
 
Highly similar visual dependencies of perceptual suppression in the absence of saccades. (A) Psychometric curve of perceptual detectability from condition 4, in which a saccade-like displacement of a vertical luminance edge took place (conceptually similar to condition 3 with real saccades). For reference, the black curve shows the psychometric curve from condition 1 (real saccades over a uniform background) at 40 ms. The saccade-like image displacement caused large suppression (a larger rightward shift of the curve), similar to the large suppression seen with real saccades (e.g., Figure 4). Error bands denote SEM across subjects. (B) Time course of perceptual suppression in condition 4. For reference, the time course of saccadic suppression over a uniform background (condition 1) is also shown in black. (C) There was also stronger suppression with (negative polarity) flashes over a dark background than with the same flashes over a bright background, exactly like with real saccades (e.g., Figure 5). The average psychometric curve was shifted more to the right with a dark background (solid). (D) The dependence of perceptual suppression on background luminance was also clear in the whole time course, and this dependence was similar to that seen with real saccades in a similar visual condition of crossing an edge (e.g., Figure 5). For reference, the black curves show the results obtained with real saccades across a uniform dark (solid) or bright (dashed) background (same as in Figure 6).
Second, we confirmed that there was stronger perceptual suppression over a dark background versus over a bright background with our simulated saccades of condition 4. This effect can be seen in Figure 7C, in which we plotted the average psychometric curves across all subjects at the same flash time, but with flashes occurring over either a bright or dark background. As we did above for simplicity in several other figures, we only plotted the curves for negative polarity flashes (but see below for explicit analysis of flash polarity in this condition as well). The psychometric curve for dark backgrounds (replicated from Figure 7A) was shifted farther to the right compared to the curve for bright backgrounds, consistent with stronger perceptual suppression. In Figure 7D, the full-time courses can be seen for both background luminances. For reference, the curves for saccadic suppression with real saccades over a uniform background are also shown in black (condition 1). At the first time point after visual transient onset in the simulated saccade condition, threshold contrast was 0.54 ± 0.11 SEM and 0.27 ± 0.03 SEM Weber contrast for dark and bright backgrounds, respectively (paired t-test: t(5) = 4.5, p = 0.006). 
Finally, we also confirmed that the same interactions between flash polarity and background luminance also persisted with simulated saccade-like image shifts. Specifically, we used condition 4 to explore psychometric curves of perceptual detectability when flashes appeared at different times relative to image displacement onset and on different background luminances. Figure 8A shows such curves for the case of negative or positive polarity flashes appearing over a dark background 47 ms after the onset of rapid image displacement. The curve for the negative polarity flashes was shifted more to the right than the curve for positive polarity flashes, suggesting stronger perceptual suppression. This is similar to what we also saw with real saccades (Figure 6). Indeed, the time courses (Figures 8B, C) exhibited very similar dependencies on flash polarity to the real saccade conditions. For example, over a dark background (Figure 8B), perceptual thresholds were at 0.54 ± 0.11 SEM Weber contrast for negative polarity flashes, and 0.35 ± 0.04 SEM for positive polarity flashes, at the time closest to peak suppression in our data (47 ms). This was statistically significant (paired t-test: t(5) = 2.7, p = 0.04). For reference, Figure 8B also shows the thresholds from condition 1 with real saccades over a uniform background. As can be seen, the suppression was significantly stronger with translation of a luminance edge across the retina than with real saccades over a uniform background. In fact, the threshold values in condition 4 (Figures 78) immediately after simulated saccade onset were generally similar in strength to those obtained immediately after real saccades in condition 3 (e.g., Figures 45); however, we caution against direct quantitative comparison, especially given the very different time courses of real versus simulated saccadic suppression profiles that can exist (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). 
Figure 8.
 
Stronger suppression for negative polarity flashes over dark backgrounds when compared to positive polarity flashes, even in the absence of saccades. (A) Average psychometric curves for negative and positive polarity flashes occurring at 47 ms after image translation onset and appearing over a dark background. Saturated colors show results with negative polarity flashes, and unsaturated colors show results with positive polarity flashes. Error bands denote SEM across subjects. Negative polarity flashes were associated with stronger perceptual suppression than positive polarity flashes. (B) Perceptual thresholds across subjects as a function of flash time after image translation onset in condition 4, comparing negative and positive polarity flashes over a dark background (error bars denote SEM across subjects). For reference, the time course of suppression from condition 1 is also shown in black. Consistent with Figure 7, perceptual suppression with an image translation (condition 4) was much stronger (higher perceptual thresholds) than in condition 1 with real saccades (compare colored to black curves). Moreover, perceptual saccadic suppression was stronger with negative polarity flashes than positive polarity flashes, especially near the time of peak suppression (47 ms). This is identical to the effect that we saw with real saccades in Figure 6. (C) With bright backgrounds, the effects of flash polarity were absent, again consistent with what we observed with real saccades (Figure 6).
Figure 8.
 
Stronger suppression for negative polarity flashes over dark backgrounds when compared to positive polarity flashes, even in the absence of saccades. (A) Average psychometric curves for negative and positive polarity flashes occurring at 47 ms after image translation onset and appearing over a dark background. Saturated colors show results with negative polarity flashes, and unsaturated colors show results with positive polarity flashes. Error bands denote SEM across subjects. Negative polarity flashes were associated with stronger perceptual suppression than positive polarity flashes. (B) Perceptual thresholds across subjects as a function of flash time after image translation onset in condition 4, comparing negative and positive polarity flashes over a dark background (error bars denote SEM across subjects). For reference, the time course of suppression from condition 1 is also shown in black. Consistent with Figure 7, perceptual suppression with an image translation (condition 4) was much stronger (higher perceptual thresholds) than in condition 1 with real saccades (compare colored to black curves). Moreover, perceptual saccadic suppression was stronger with negative polarity flashes than positive polarity flashes, especially near the time of peak suppression (47 ms). This is identical to the effect that we saw with real saccades in Figure 6. (C) With bright backgrounds, the effects of flash polarity were absent, again consistent with what we observed with real saccades (Figure 6).
The interaction between flash polarity and background luminance also extended to bright backgrounds. Specifically, with real saccades in conditions 2 and 3, we saw above (Figure 6) that flash polarity did not alter saccadic suppression strength when flashes appeared over a bright background, unlike the case with flashes occurring over a dark background. This happened in a similar way in condition 4 with simulated saccades (Figure 8C). Therefore, all of the visual dependencies that we observed with real saccades (Figures 36) also occurred in the absence of any saccades, when saccade-like image translations were introduced (Figures 78). 
It should be noted here that our simulated saccade condition was also important because it allowed us to additionally test probe flashes occurring before simulated saccade onset. In Figures 78, flashes with negative time were presented before the image translation. Nonetheless, they were still associated with threshold elevations, as we also recently reported in similar experiments (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). For example, in Figure 7B, perceptual threshold for a flash time of –24 ms was 0.11 Weber contrast, whereas it was only 0.09 at –35 ms. Therefore, there was elevation of threshold as time approached the onset of the image translation. These results, combined with earlier published work in the literature, make it likely that we would also observe pre-saccadic suppression in conditions 1, 2, and 3 above if we were to present flashes before the real saccades. Interestingly, even pre-translation flashes were clearly associated with stronger suppression when they occurred over a dark background as opposed to a bright background (Figure 7D), consistent with the post-translation flash times demonstrating an influence of background luminance, which are in turn consistent with real saccade background effects. 
Luminance steps without saccade-like image sweeps cause modest perceptual suppression
Finally, for completeness, we also tested simple luminance steps during fixation, with no image translations (condition 5; Methods). In this case, we previously found that perceptual suppression does occur (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020), although in that previous study, we did not explore certain factors, like flash polarity, in much detail. 
With luminance steps, perceptual suppression was significantly weaker than with image translations. For example, Figure 9A shows thresholds when a flash occurred over a dark background, and Figure 9B shows thresholds when a flash occurred over a bright background. For reference, saccadic suppression thresholds from condition 1 (i.e., with saccades across a uniform background) are also shown in black. Both conditions had the weakest overall suppression strengths in the whole study, and peak suppression in them was very similar. For example, at the first flash time after contrast change or saccade onset, the overall thresholds in both conditions were less than 0.18 Weber contrast (Figure 9), which is much less than threshold contrasts in all other experiments with luminance edges or stripes (whether with or without saccades). This suggests an interesting role of luminance transients over perifoveal image regions in perceptual saccadic suppression. Specifically, perifoveally, the uniformity of the display was similar during the real saccades (condition 1) and the contrast change experiment (condition 5); both conditions were lacking local image translation across the retinal regions that were also probed with brief flash stimuli. Even though the entire retinal image (including display outer edges) was translated across the retina during real saccades, it seems that such a local image translation is necessary to maximize perceptual suppression of temporally close probe flashes. 
Figure 9.
 
Weak perceptual suppression with simple luminance steps during fixation. (A) Perceptual contrast thresholds from condition 5 with just luminance steps (colored curves). Each curve shows results for either a positive (dark green) or negative (light green) polarity flash over a dark background, and error bars denote SEM across subjects. For reference, thresholds from condition 1 are shown in black. Peak suppression with a luminance step was as weak as the suppression with real saccades over a uniform background (this was our weakest suppression condition in all experiments; Figures 18). Moreover, during recovery, negative polarity flashes were harder to detect than positive polarity flashes. (B) Similar results for flashes occurring over a bright background. Now, it was positive polarity flashes that were harder to detect than negative polarity flashes during the recovery phase (at times 71 and 106 ms after luminance step occurrence). All other conventions are similar to Figures 45.
Figure 9.
 
Weak perceptual suppression with simple luminance steps during fixation. (A) Perceptual contrast thresholds from condition 5 with just luminance steps (colored curves). Each curve shows results for either a positive (dark green) or negative (light green) polarity flash over a dark background, and error bars denote SEM across subjects. For reference, thresholds from condition 1 are shown in black. Peak suppression with a luminance step was as weak as the suppression with real saccades over a uniform background (this was our weakest suppression condition in all experiments; Figures 18). Moreover, during recovery, negative polarity flashes were harder to detect than positive polarity flashes. (B) Similar results for flashes occurring over a bright background. Now, it was positive polarity flashes that were harder to detect than negative polarity flashes during the recovery phase (at times 71 and 106 ms after luminance step occurrence). All other conventions are similar to Figures 45.
Figure 9 also reveals interesting differential effects of flash polarity on perceptual thresholds, but only at longer times after contrast change. Specifically, at both 71 ms and 106 ms after background luminance change (from bright to dark or vice versa), there was an interaction between flash polarity and background luminance: negative polarity flashes were harder to detect compared to positive polarity flashes over dark backgrounds, whereas positive polarity flashes over bright backgrounds were harder to detect than negative polarity flashes. To statistically validate this, we pooled the two latest flash times together and compared, with a paired t-test for each background, whether the detection thresholds differed for the two flash polarities. With a dark background, the positive polarity (mean threshold: 0.07 ± 0.01 SEM Weber contrast) significantly differed from the negative polarity (mean threshold: 0.09 ± 0.01 SEM) (t(5) = 5.8, p = 0.0022). Over a bright background, the positive polarity (threshold: 0.09 ± 0.01 SEM Weber contrast) significantly differed from the negative polarity (0.06 ± 0.01 SEM) (t(5) = –16, p < 10−4). This effect was not present in any of our real or simulated saccade conditions (conditions 1–4), and it also might be a simple instantiation of Weber's law, irrespective of perceptual suppression. Interestingly, in our recent investigation of a similar perceptual paradigm (Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020), there was no clear impact of flash polarity. However, in that study, we did not use the more sensitive psychometric approach that we used here, so we could have been affected by floor and ceiling effects in that earlier study. Nonetheless, it is interesting that visual-visual interactions in saccadic suppression are best matched under fixation when saccade-like image displacements take place; other transients, such as just simple background luminance steps (Figure 9), also cause mild suppression, but with different visual dependencies. 
Discussion
We investigated the visual components of perceptual saccadic suppression. We collected psychometric curves of perceptual detectability for brief peri-saccadic flashes and used these curves to estimate thresholds. We found that perceptual thresholds were only elevated mildly when saccades were made across a uniform background, but they were dramatically increased when gaze crossed a luminance edge (e.g., Figures 34). Moreover, the luminance at which gaze landed after saccade end did not have to be different to the luminance at saccade onset for the strength of saccadic suppression to increase; crossing a luminance stripe was also associated with very strong saccadic suppression when compared to the uniform background (condition 2, e.g., Figure 4). Interestingly, saccadic suppression was the strongest when the luminance of the background was dark, and there was an impact of flash polarity relative to the background luminance (particularly with negative polarity flashes over dark backgrounds; e.g., Figures 56). Critically, all of these visual dependencies of saccadic suppression still occurred when we replaced saccades with image displacements in the absence of any saccadic eye movements (e.g., Figures 78). 
Our results from condition 3 confirm and extend those of (Maij et al., 2012). In that study, the authors used a condition similar to ours (Figure 1A, bottom), and they asked subjects to indicate whether they missed seeing a peri-saccadic flash or not (the overall context of the task was to study mislocalization of observed flash location, so the flashes were supra-threshold in general compared to ours). When the saccades were made over a uniform background, the subjects almost never missed the flashes. However, when the saccades involved gaze crossing a vertical luminance edge, there was an increased likelihood of misses. This is also what we saw with our more sensitive threshold measurements in the current study (e.g., Figures 34). As stated above, we also saw almost equally strong suppression when the edge was replaced with just a luminance stripe (condition 2, i.e., the starting and landing gaze positions were on the same background luminance; Figures 34). Combined, these results suggest that visual-visual interactions, in the form of gaze crossing a luminance pattern (one vertical edge in condition 3 or two vertical edges in condition 3), matter a great deal for saccadic suppression. 
Moreover, the effects are highly repeatable across individuals. In all analyses, average measurements across subjects were very consistent and repeatable, as evidenced by the small error bars in all our figures. Indeed, when we plotted the individual psychometric curves of each subject for one of the conditions, as in the example of Figure 10, we saw remarkable repeatability across subjects. This was true in our earlier study as well (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). Therefore, the effects that we report here are clearly robust across individuals. 
Figure 10.
 
Raw psychometric curves at two flash times from each individual subject in condition 2. (A) Psychometric curves of all subjects with a negative polarity flash at 40 ms and dark backgrounds. (B) The same for the flash at 63 ms. There was strong coherence across subjects in the modulations of perceptual detectability in our experiments.
Figure 10.
 
Raw psychometric curves at two flash times from each individual subject in condition 2. (A) Psychometric curves of all subjects with a negative polarity flash at 40 ms and dark backgrounds. (B) The same for the flash at 63 ms. There was strong coherence across subjects in the modulations of perceptual detectability in our experiments.
It is also interesting that the background luminance has a strong influence on the strength of saccadic suppression, as was also suggested previously (Brooks & Fuchs, 1975; Brooks et al., 1980). In all analyses, we assessed perceptual thresholds based on calculating the absolute luminance difference between the probe flash and the background, normalized against the background luminance (i.e., Weber contrast). We observed that subjects needed more luminance contrast relative to the background (even with positive polarity flashes) to detect stimuli peri-saccadically (or peri-translation in condition 4) when the overall background was dark than when it was light. The fact that this effect was stronger with the dark background compared to the bright one rules out the possibility that this was a simple instantiation of basic Weber's law (i.e., that more luminance needs to be added to a bright background than to a dark background for equal detectability). Rather, there was an apparent interaction between image translation and gaze finally encountering an image patch with a dark region. Similarly, there was also an interaction associated with the probe polarity itself: after saccades (or image translations), dark backgrounds required larger negative polarity contrasts for perceptual detection than positive polarity contrasts, but no polarity effects existed with a bright background (e.g., Figure 6). It could be the case that crossing an image patch to a darker background induces a stronger visual transient (e.g., in the retina) than crossing an image patch to a brighter background, which then modifies the subsequent visual response to the probe flash itself. 
It would be interesting to explore this mechanism by relating how our observed interaction effects (that is, perceptually) relate to the activity of ON and OFF channels in ex-vivo mouse and pig retinae, similar to our recent investigations (Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020). Intriguingly, preliminary data from that study indicate that ex-vivo non-human primate retinae may exhibit ON and OFF dependencies that might be similar to human effects (Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020), at least in our condition 5. On the other hand, our probe flash polarity effects (e.g., Figures 68) demonstrate that strong perceptual suppression can occur irrespective of the combination of visual transient polarity and probe flash polarity; this suggests the presence of other suppressive mechanisms beyond the dynamic reversal suppression mechanism (which depends on a difference in polarity between visual transient and probe flash) that we uncovered in the mouse and pig retinae in the same study. 
Of course, it would also be interesting to investigate the visual-visual interactions of saccadic suppression in neural circuits downstream of the retina in non-human primates (as well as other relevant species used in systems neuroscience). Indeed, neurons preferring “black” stimuli are prevalent in the primary visual cortex (Yeh, Xing, & Shapley, 2009), and sensory processing times are also different for dark versus bright stimuli (Jin, Wang, Lashgari, Swadlow, & Alonso, 2011; Komban et al., 2014). Moreover, there are asymmetric interactions between background luminance and the effectiveness of ON and OFF early visual processing channels (Rahimi-Nasrabadi et al., 2021). Finally, dark versus bright stimuli can have differential effects on visuo-motor processing in monkeys (Malevich, Buonocore, & Hafed, 2020). All of these results, combined with our current study, motivate future experiments on sensory-motor processing with dark stimuli in brain areas downstream of the retina. 
The importance of visual-visual interactions in perceptual saccadic suppression also makes us consider the possibility that with our uniform background condition (condition 1), the edges of the monitor may have had a non-negligible modulatory effect on the phenomenon. Specifically, for both the bright and dark background luminances used in the uniform background condition, the display monitor was still brighter than the rest of the laboratory. Therefore, there was a rectangular frame that was moved on the retina by the saccades. This likely affected perception since our other conditions (with luminance edges crossing the fovea) had a very strong effect on the strength of saccadic suppression. Nonetheless, it is still intriguing that, by far, the largest effects on peri-saccadic perceptual thresholds that we observed required that the luminance edges or patterns cross the fovea during the saccades. Such foveal crossing (or, more generally, crossing of the retinal patch experiencing the probe flashes) also happened in our simulated saccade condition (condition 4), which again showed strong perceptual suppression effects. This could be a function of the spatial resolution of the retinal regions (and associated downstream visual areas) that experience temporal luminance modulations during the saccades and the flashes. 
In addition to the above effects in real saccades, we also saw essentially the same visual dependencies in experiments with visual-visual interactions no longer involving real saccades. Specifically, in our condition 4, we translated the luminance edge across the fovea and presented probe flashes near the time of the translation. We observed the same dependencies on background luminance, and the same interactions with flash luminance polarity, as with real saccades. This confirms our hypothesis (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020; Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020) that visual-visual interactions play an important role in saccadic suppression, and it also extends the image conditions under which we could observe remarkable similarities between real and simulated saccades in terms of perceptual suppression (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). All of this is in agreement with other phenomena, like the fact that intra-saccadic motion can still be perceived if its speed is within the detectability range of our motion sensors in the brain (Castet et al., 2001; Castet & Masson, 2000), and also the fact that intra-saccadic motion streaks provide important reference frames for keeping track of object locations (Schweitzer & Rolfs, 2020). This evidence highlights the role of vision even during rapid eye movements. In that sense, suppression occurring before saccades or image translations (e.g., Figures 79) may be viewed as reflecting visual-visual interactions such as backward masking (Breitmeyer, 2007; Brooks & Fuchs, 1975; Judge, Wurtz, & Richmond, 1980; Mackay, 1970; Mitrani et al., 1975). 
Naturally, a role for prior knowledge of movement commands at the time of saccade generation (e.g., corollary discharge) must additionally matter for saccadic suppression, and the real question becomes how visual and non-visual mechanisms interact in this phenomenon. In other words, it need not be the case that saccadic suppression is only purely motor or only purely visual. For example, experiments under conditions of whiteout (i.e., absolutely uniform illumination across the entire retina) might suggest a role for a movement-related impact on perceptual saccadic suppression (Riggs & Manning, 1982). Interestingly, at the neural level, enhancement, rather than suppression, seems to take place in human LGN and V1 across saccades without visual stimulation (Sylvester, Haynes, & Rees, 2005; Sylvester & Rees, 2006). This suggests a very different role for knowledge of saccade commands than simply to actively reduce visual sensitivity. Similarly, we recently found that while the visual properties of perceptual suppression were highly similar with or without real saccades, there was a massive difference in the time course of suppression when real saccades were generated: perceptual suppression was much more short-lived when it was associated with real saccades than when it was triggered by visual-visual interactions in the absence of saccades (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020). This is very reassuring, in retrospect, because of the strong need to minimize saccade-induced disruptions in vision as much as possible. However, it also raises highly interesting and unanswered questions on what exactly the role of saccade-related movement commands is in modulating the properties of perceptual saccadic suppression. Could it be that corollary discharge is there to shorten saccadic suppression, rather than to cause it? This is a topic that we think will lead to very interesting new neurophysiological insights on the field of trans-saccadic perception. 
Conclusions
The phenomenon of perceptual saccadic suppression possesses a strong visual component, reflecting interactions between visual activation caused by saccade-induced image translations and visual activation caused by the brief probe flashes used to measure the sensitivity of the visual system around the time of saccades. This visual component of saccadic suppression motivates future neurophysiological, perceptual, and theoretical investigations on how this component interacts with internal knowledge of saccadic movement generation commands (i.e., efference copies or corollary discharge) for minimizing the disruptions to vision that can be caused by saccades. 
Acknowledgments
MPB and ZMH were funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) Collaborative Research Center SFB 1233 on “Robust Vision” (project number: 276693517). SI and TAM were funded by DFG grant number MU3792/3-1. 
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Matthias P. Baumann. 
Email: matthias-philipp.baumann@student.uni-tuebingen.de. 
Address: Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, Tübingen University, Tübingen, Germany. 
References
Adey, W. R., & Noda, H. (1973). Influence of eye movements on geniculo-striate excitability in the cat. Journal of Physiology, 235(3), 805–821, https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010418. [CrossRef]
Beeler, G. W., Jr. (1967). Visual threshold changes resulting from spontaneous saccadic eye movements. Vision Research, 7(9), 769–775. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bellet, J., Chen, C. Y., & Hafed, Z. M. (2017). Sequential hemifield gating of alpha- and beta-behavioral performance oscillations after microsaccades. Journal of Neurophysiology, 118(5), 2789–2805, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00253.2017. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bellet, M. E., Bellet, J., Nienborg, H., Hafed, Z. M., & Berens, P. (2019). Human-level saccade detection performance using deep neural networks. Journal of Neurophysiology, 121(2), 646–661, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00601.2018. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Benedetto, A., & Morrone, M. C. (2017). Saccadic Suppression Is Embedded Within Extended Oscillatory Modulation of Sensitivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(13), 3661–3670, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2390-16.2016. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Berman, R. A., Cavanaugh, J., McAlonan, K., & Wurtz, R. H. (2017). A circuit for saccadic suppression in the primate brain. Journal of Neurophysiology, 117(4), 1720–1735, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00679.2016. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Binda, P., & Morrone, M. C. (2018). Vision During Saccadic Eye Movements. Annu Rev Vis Sci, 4, 193–213, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034317. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bogadhi, A. R., Buonocore, A., & Hafed, Z. M. (2020). Task-irrelevant visual forms facilitate covert and overt spatial selection. Journal of Neuroscience, 40(49), 9496–9506, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1593-20.2020. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Breitmeyer, B. G. (2007). Visual masking: past accomplishments, present status, future developments. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3(1), 9–20. [CrossRef]
Bremmer, F., Kubischik, M., Hoffmann, K. P., & Krekelberg, B. (2009). Neural dynamics of saccadic suppression. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(40), 12374–12383, https://doi.org/29/40/12374 [pii] 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2908-09.2009. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Brooks, B. A., & Fuchs, A. F. (1975). Influence of stimulus parameters on visual sensitivity during saccadic eye movement. Vision Research, 15(12), 1389–1398, https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(75)90196-0. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Brooks, B. A., Impelman, D. M., & Lum, J. T. (1980). Influence of background luminance on visual sensitivity during saccadic eye movements. Experimental Brain Research Experimentelle Hirnforschung Experimentation Cerebrale, 40(3), 322–329, https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00237797. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Brooks, B. A., Impelman, D. M., & Lum, J. T. (1981). Backward and forward masking associated with saccadic eye movement. Perception and Psychophysics, 30(1), 62–70. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Burr, D. C., Morrone, M. C., & Ross, J. (1994). Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements. Nature, 371(6497), 511–513, https://doi.org/10.1038/371511a0. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Castet, E., Jeanjean, S., & Masson, G. S. (2001). 'Saccadic suppression' — no need for an active extra-retinal mechanism. Trends in Neuroscience (Tins), 24(6), 316–318. [CrossRef]
Castet, E., & Masson, G. S. (2000). Motion perception during saccadic eye movements. Nature Neuroscience, 3(2), 177–183, https://doi.org/10.1038/72124. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chen, C. Y., & Hafed, Z. M. (2013). Postmicrosaccadic enhancement of slow eye movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(12), 5375–5386, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3703-12.2013. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chen, C. Y., & Hafed, Z. M. (2017). A neural locus for spatial-frequency specific saccadic suppression in visual-motor neurons of the primate superior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology, 117(4), 1657–1673, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00911.2016. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chen, C. Y., Ignashchenkova, A., Thier, P., & Hafed, Z. M. (2015). Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades. Current Biology, 25(16), 2065–2074, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.022. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cornelissen, F. W., Peters, E. M., & Palmer, J. (2002). The Eyelink Toolbox: eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers: A Journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc, 34(4), 613–617, https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03195489.
DeYoe, E. A., & Van Essen, D. C. (1988). Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex. Trends in Neuroscience (Tins), 11(5), 219–226, https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(88)90130-0. [CrossRef]
Diamond, M. R., Ross, J., & Morrone, M. C. (2000). Extraretinal control of saccadic suppression. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(9), 3449–3455. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Duffy, F. H., & Lombroso, C. T. (1968). Electrophysiological evidence for visual suppression prior to the onset of a voluntary saccadic eye movement. Nature, 218(5146), 1074–1075, https://doi.org/10.1038/2181074a0. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gremmler, S., & Lappe, M. (2017). Saccadic suppression during voluntary versus reactive saccades. Journal of Vision, 17(8), 8, https://doi.org/10.1167/17.8.8. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hafed, Z. M. (2013). Alteration of visual perception prior to microsaccades. Neuron, 77(4), 775–786, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.014. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hafed, Z. M., Chen, C.-Y., & Tian, X. (2015). Vision, perception, and attention through the lens of microsaccades: mechanisms and implications. Front Syst Neurosci, 9, 167, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2015.00167. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hafed, Z. M., & Krauzlis, R. J. (2010). Microsaccadic suppression of visual bursts in the primate superior colliculus. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(28), 9542–9547, https://doi.org/30/28/9542 [pii] 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1137-10.2010. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hass, C. A., & Horwitz, G. D. (2011). Effects of microsaccades on contrast detection and V1 responses in macaques. Journal of Vision (Charlottesville, Va.), 11(3), 1–17, https://doi.org/10.1167/11.3.3.
Idrees, S., Baumann, M. P., Franke, F., Munch, T. A., & Hafed, Z. M. (2020). Perceptual saccadic suppression starts in the retina. Nature communications, 11(1), 1977, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-15890-w. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Idrees, S., Baumann, M. P., Korympidou, M. M., Schubert, T., Kling, A., Franke, K., . . . Munch, T. A. (2020). Suppression without inhibition: A novel mechanism in the retina accounts for saccadic suppression. bioRxiv, https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.08.21.261198.
Jin, J., Wang, Y., Lashgari, R., Swadlow, H. A., & Alonso, J. M. (2011). Faster thalamocortical processing for dark than light visual targets. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(48), 17471–17479, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2456-11.2011. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Judge, S. J., Wurtz, R. H., & Richmond, B. J. (1980). Vision during saccadic eye movements. I. Visual interactions in striate cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 43(4), 1133–1155. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kagan, I., Gur, M., & Snodderly, D. M. (2008). Saccades and drifts differentially modulate neuronal activity in V1: effects of retinal image motion, position, and extraretinal influences. Journal of Vision (Charlottesville, Va.), 8(14), 19 11–25, https://doi.org/10.1167/8.14.19.
Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., & Pelli, D. G. (2007). What's new in Psychtoolbox-3? (Abstract). Perception, 36(14), 1–16.
Komban, S. J., Kremkow, J., Jin, J., Wang, Y., Lashgari, R., Li, X., &hellip Alonso, J. M. (2014). Neuronal and perceptual differences in the temporal processing of darks and lights. Neuron, 82(1), 224–234, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.020. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Latour, P. L. (1962). Visual threshold during eye movements. Vision Research, 2(3), 261–262. [CrossRef]
Mackay, D. M. (1970). Elevation of visual threshold by displacement of retinal image. Nature, 225(5227), 90–92, https://doi.org/10.1038/225090a0. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Macknik, S. L., & Livingstone, M. S. (1998). Neuronal correlates of visibility and invisibility in the primate visual system. Nature Neuroscience, 1(2), 144–149, https://doi.org/10.1038/393. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Maij, F., Matziridi, M., Smeets, J. B., & Brenner, E. (2012). Luminance contrast in the background makes flashes harder to detect during saccades. Vision Research, 60, 22–27, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2012.03.003. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Malevich, T., Buonocore, A., & Hafed, Z. M. (2020). Rapid stimulus-driven modulation of slow ocular position drifts. Elife, 9:e57595, https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.57595.
Matin, E. (1974). Saccadic suppression: a review and an analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 81(12), 899–917. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Melcher, D. (2011). Visual stability. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 366(1564), 468–475, https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0277. [CrossRef]
Merigan, W. H., & Maunsell, J. H. (1993). How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 369–402, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.002101. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mitrani, L., Mateeff, S., & Yakimoff, N. (1971). Is Saccadic Suppression Really Saccadic. Vision Research, 11(10), 1157–1161, https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(71)90119-2. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mitrani, L., Radil-Weiss, T., Yakimoff, N., Mateeff, S., & Bozkov, V. (1975). Various background pattern-effect on saccadic suppression. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 17(3), 161–164. [PubMed]
Mitrani, L., Yakimoff, N., & Mateeff, S. (1973). Saccadic suppression in the presence of structured background. Vision Research, 13(2), 517–521. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Noda, H., & Adey, W. R. (1974). Retinal ganglion cells of the cat transfer information on saccadic eye movement and quick target motion. Brain Research, 70(2), 340–345, https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(74)90323-0. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial vision, 10(4), 437–442. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rahimi-Nasrabadi, H., Jin, J., Mazade, R., Pons, C., Najafian, S., & Alonso, J. M. (2021). Image luminance changes contrast sensitivity in visual cortex. Cell reports, 34(5), 108692, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2021.108692. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Reppas, J. B., Usrey, W. M., & Reid, R. C. (2002). Saccadic eye movements modulate visual responses in the lateral geniculate nucleus. Neuron, 35(5), 961–974, https://doi.org/S0896627302008231 [pii]. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Riggs, L. A., & Manning, K. A. (1982). Saccadic suppression under conditions of whiteout. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 23(1), 138–143. [PubMed]
Robinson, D. L., & Wurtz, R. H. (1976). Use of an extraretinal signal by monkey superior colliculus neurons to distinguish real from self-induced stimulus movement. Journal of Neurophysiology, 39(4), 852–870. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ross, J., Morrone, M. C., Goldberg, M. E., & Burr, D. C. (2001). Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades. Trends in Neuroscience (Tins), 24(2), 113–121, https://doi.org/S0166-2236(00)01685-4 [pii]. [CrossRef]
Scholes, C., McGraw, P. V., & Roach, N. W. (2018). Selective modulation of visual sensitivity during fixation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 119(6), 2059–2067, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00819.2017. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schutt, H. H., Harmeling, S., Macke, J. H., & Wichmann, F. A. (2016). Painfree and accurate Bayesian estimation of psychometric functions for (potentially) overdispersed data. Vision Research, 122, 105–123, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2016.02.002. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schweitzer, R., & Rolfs, M. (2020). Intra-saccadic motion streaks as cues to linking object locations across saccades. Journal of Vision (Charlottesville, Va.), 20(4), 17, https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.4.17. [CrossRef]
Sylvester, R., Haynes, J. D., & Rees, G. (2005). Saccades differentially modulate human LGN and V1 responses in the presence and absence of visual stimulation. Current Biology, 15(1), 37–41, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2004.12.061. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sylvester, R., & Rees, G. (2006). Extraretinal saccadic signals in human LGN and early retinotopic cortex. Neuroimage, 30(1), 214–219, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.014. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Watson, A. B., & Pelli, D. G. (1983). QUEST: a Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Perception and Psychophysics, 33(2), 113–120. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wurtz, R. H. (2008). Neuronal mechanisms of visual stability. Vision Research, 48(20), 2070–2089, https://doi.org/S0042-6989(08)00172-7 [pii] 10.1016/j.visres.2008.03.021. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wurtz, R. H., Joiner, W. M., & Berman, R. A. (2011). Neuronal mechanisms for visual stability: progress and problems. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 366(1564), 492–503, https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0186. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Yakimoff, N., Mitrani, L., & Mateeff, S. (1974). Saccadic suppression as visual masking effect. Agressologie, 15(6), 387–394. [PubMed]
Yeh, C. I., Xing, D., & Shapley, R. M. (2009). “Black” responses dominate macaque primary visual cortex v1. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(38), 11753–11760, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1991-09.2009. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Zimmermann, E., & Bremmer, F. (2016). Visual Neuroscience: The Puzzle of Perceptual Stability. Current Biology, 26(5), R199–201, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.01.050. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Zuber, B. L., & Stark, L. (1966). Saccadic suppression: elevation of visual threshold associated with saccadic eye movements. Experimental Neurology, 16(1), 65–79, https://doi.org/0014-4886(66)90087-2 [pii]. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
The different saccade conditions tested in our study. (A) Subjects made approximately 11.8 degrees saccades towards display center from one of four possible locations. The saccades (schematized as curved arrows) were predominantly horizontal (see text), and we detected their onset online using a velocity criterion. Upon saccade detection, we presented a brief luminance flash at 7 degrees either above or below the saccade target (right column of images). The flash was presented at one of four different delays from online saccade detection (see B–D). In condition 1 (top row), the saccades were made over a uniform background. In condition 2 (middle row), gaze crossed a vertical luminance stripe. And, in condition 3 (bottom row), the saccades brought gaze across a vertical luminance edge. We varied background luminance or the combination of pre- and post-saccadic background luminances across trials, and the probe flashes could also be of either positive (luminance increments relative to the background) or negative (luminance decrements) polarity. (B) After detecting saccades offline, we plotted the distributions of flash times collected during the experiments in condition 1 across all subjects. Our experimental manipulation succeeded in probing multiple times of perceptual sensitivity after saccade onset. The jitter in the individual distributions was due to variability in online saccade detection, as well as display update timing variability (due to the asynchrony between saccades and display refresh clocks). The solid vertical lines show the means of each distribution. (C, D) Similar distributions of flash times for conditions 2 and 3, suggesting that differences in perceptual thresholds across conditions (see Results) were not attributable to different flash times relative to saccade onset. The vertical lines (dashed in C and dotted in D) show the average flash times for each colored distribution in the respective panel, and they are extended upward towards the distributions in B to facilitate comparison of mean flash times. The mean times (of each colored distribution) were very similar across conditions (with a maximal observed difference of 0.8 ms).
Figure 1.
 
The different saccade conditions tested in our study. (A) Subjects made approximately 11.8 degrees saccades towards display center from one of four possible locations. The saccades (schematized as curved arrows) were predominantly horizontal (see text), and we detected their onset online using a velocity criterion. Upon saccade detection, we presented a brief luminance flash at 7 degrees either above or below the saccade target (right column of images). The flash was presented at one of four different delays from online saccade detection (see B–D). In condition 1 (top row), the saccades were made over a uniform background. In condition 2 (middle row), gaze crossed a vertical luminance stripe. And, in condition 3 (bottom row), the saccades brought gaze across a vertical luminance edge. We varied background luminance or the combination of pre- and post-saccadic background luminances across trials, and the probe flashes could also be of either positive (luminance increments relative to the background) or negative (luminance decrements) polarity. (B) After detecting saccades offline, we plotted the distributions of flash times collected during the experiments in condition 1 across all subjects. Our experimental manipulation succeeded in probing multiple times of perceptual sensitivity after saccade onset. The jitter in the individual distributions was due to variability in online saccade detection, as well as display update timing variability (due to the asynchrony between saccades and display refresh clocks). The solid vertical lines show the means of each distribution. (C, D) Similar distributions of flash times for conditions 2 and 3, suggesting that differences in perceptual thresholds across conditions (see Results) were not attributable to different flash times relative to saccade onset. The vertical lines (dashed in C and dotted in D) show the average flash times for each colored distribution in the respective panel, and they are extended upward towards the distributions in B to facilitate comparison of mean flash times. The mean times (of each colored distribution) were very similar across conditions (with a maximal observed difference of 0.8 ms).
Figure 2.
 
The visual-only conditions tested in our study. (A) In condition 4, subjects fixated the center of the display, and the background was identical to that of condition 3 in Figure 1. After initial fixation was established, the whole background translated horizontally to shift one of the two vertical edges across gaze position. After the translation, gaze was now aimed at a different background luminance. This condition mimicked the real saccade version in condition 3. Note that during translation, we treated the texture as being horizontally periodic (in the shown example, this meant that a bright background was revealed on the right edge of the display monitor during leftward translation). At different times relative to translation onset, we presented a flash either above or below fixation, similar to Figure 1A. In this case, the flashes could be presented either before or after translation onset (Methods). When presented after translation onset, the timing was such that they were presented over the new background luminance after translation end (that is, the edge had crossed display center even for the earliest flash time after translation onset). (B) In condition 5, the subjects fixated the center of a uniform display, and we changed the luminance of the background (from dark to bright or vice versa). Relative to this transient, a probe flash could happen at similar times to those used in A. Note that in this condition only, we had four possible probe flash locations, because this experiment was a replication of our earlier version of it in (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020; Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020).
Figure 2.
 
The visual-only conditions tested in our study. (A) In condition 4, subjects fixated the center of the display, and the background was identical to that of condition 3 in Figure 1. After initial fixation was established, the whole background translated horizontally to shift one of the two vertical edges across gaze position. After the translation, gaze was now aimed at a different background luminance. This condition mimicked the real saccade version in condition 3. Note that during translation, we treated the texture as being horizontally periodic (in the shown example, this meant that a bright background was revealed on the right edge of the display monitor during leftward translation). At different times relative to translation onset, we presented a flash either above or below fixation, similar to Figure 1A. In this case, the flashes could be presented either before or after translation onset (Methods). When presented after translation onset, the timing was such that they were presented over the new background luminance after translation end (that is, the edge had crossed display center even for the earliest flash time after translation onset). (B) In condition 5, the subjects fixated the center of a uniform display, and we changed the luminance of the background (from dark to bright or vice versa). Relative to this transient, a probe flash could happen at similar times to those used in A. Note that in this condition only, we had four possible probe flash locations, because this experiment was a replication of our earlier version of it in (Idrees, Baumann, Franke, et al., 2020; Idrees, Baumann, Korympidou, et al., 2020).
Figure 3.
 
Much stronger saccadic suppression across a luminance stripe than across a uniform background. Example psychometric curves of perceptual detectability from one sample subject, for dark probe flashes over a dark background. Circles show individual data points, with the size of the circle scaled based on the numbers of observations collected. The blue curves show psychometric curves when the saccade crossed a luminance stripe (condition 2), whereas the black curves show the results obtained when the saccade was made across a uniform background (condition 1); see the schematic legend in B. Vertical lines indicate the perceptual threshold values in each condition, and horizontal error bars show 95% confidence intervals for each psychometric curve (obtained using the psignifit 4 toolbox; Methods). (A) Data for probe flashes presented immediately after online saccade detection (40 ms after actual saccade onset; first time cluster in Figures 1B–D). There was a much larger rightward shift in the psychometric curve in the case of condition 2 when compared to condition 1. (B) Data for a later flash time (63 ms after saccade onset; third time cluster in Figures 1B–D). Both curves recovered at this time point (i.e., shifted leftward relative to A; horizontal leftward arrows), but the blue curve still showed much more suppression (i.e., rightward shift) compared to the black curve.
Figure 3.
 
Much stronger saccadic suppression across a luminance stripe than across a uniform background. Example psychometric curves of perceptual detectability from one sample subject, for dark probe flashes over a dark background. Circles show individual data points, with the size of the circle scaled based on the numbers of observations collected. The blue curves show psychometric curves when the saccade crossed a luminance stripe (condition 2), whereas the black curves show the results obtained when the saccade was made across a uniform background (condition 1); see the schematic legend in B. Vertical lines indicate the perceptual threshold values in each condition, and horizontal error bars show 95% confidence intervals for each psychometric curve (obtained using the psignifit 4 toolbox; Methods). (A) Data for probe flashes presented immediately after online saccade detection (40 ms after actual saccade onset; first time cluster in Figures 1B–D). There was a much larger rightward shift in the psychometric curve in the case of condition 2 when compared to condition 1. (B) Data for a later flash time (63 ms after saccade onset; third time cluster in Figures 1B–D). Both curves recovered at this time point (i.e., shifted leftward relative to A; horizontal leftward arrows), but the blue curve still showed much more suppression (i.e., rightward shift) compared to the black curve.
Figure 4.
 
Systematically stronger saccadic suppression across a luminance stripe or luminance edge than across a uniform background. (A) Average psychometric curves of perceptual detectability from all subjects when the probe flashes occurred 40 ms after saccade onset (first time cluster in Figures 1B–D). Black: psychometric curve for condition 1, when saccades were made across a uniform background. Blue: psychometric curve when saccades crossed a luminance stripe (condition 2). Red: psychometric curve for condition 3, when saccades crossed a luminance edge. In all cases, error bands denote SEM across subjects. Perceptual thresholds were elevated much more for conditions 2 and 3 than for condition 1. (B) Same as in A but for a later time point (63 ms). There was perceptual recovery, but saccadic suppression was still stronger in conditions 2 and 3 than in condition 1; the subjects always had higher thresholds than in condition 1 (also see C). (C) Perceptual thresholds (average across subjects) for each flash time after saccade onset. Higher values mean stronger saccadic suppression. Error bars denote SEM across subjects. Note that the time values on the x-axis are the average flash times obtained after offline saccade detection (Methods and Figures 1B–D). Also note that for each average flash time, we slightly jittered the x-axis position of each data point to not mask individual data points and their error bars (also true in subsequent figures below). There was much stronger saccadic suppression in conditions 2 and 3 than in condition 1.
Figure 4.
 
Systematically stronger saccadic suppression across a luminance stripe or luminance edge than across a uniform background. (A) Average psychometric curves of perceptual detectability from all subjects when the probe flashes occurred 40 ms after saccade onset (first time cluster in Figures 1B–D). Black: psychometric curve for condition 1, when saccades were made across a uniform background. Blue: psychometric curve when saccades crossed a luminance stripe (condition 2). Red: psychometric curve for condition 3, when saccades crossed a luminance edge. In all cases, error bands denote SEM across subjects. Perceptual thresholds were elevated much more for conditions 2 and 3 than for condition 1. (B) Same as in A but for a later time point (63 ms). There was perceptual recovery, but saccadic suppression was still stronger in conditions 2 and 3 than in condition 1; the subjects always had higher thresholds than in condition 1 (also see C). (C) Perceptual thresholds (average across subjects) for each flash time after saccade onset. Higher values mean stronger saccadic suppression. Error bars denote SEM across subjects. Note that the time values on the x-axis are the average flash times obtained after offline saccade detection (Methods and Figures 1B–D). Also note that for each average flash time, we slightly jittered the x-axis position of each data point to not mask individual data points and their error bars (also true in subsequent figures below). There was much stronger saccadic suppression in conditions 2 and 3 than in condition 1.
Figure 5.
 
Stronger saccadic suppression over dark backgrounds, whether with luminance edges or luminance stripes. (A) Average psychometric curves of perceptual detectability from all subjects in condition 2, and for probes occurring 40 ms after saccade onset (i.e., the first time cluster in Figures 1B–D). Solid indicates flashes over a dark background (i.e., saccades starting and ending on a dark patch, but crossing a bright stripe), and dashed indicates flashes over a bright background (i.e., saccades starting and ending on a bright patch, but crossing a dark stripe). In both cases, the probe flashes had negative luminance polarity (i.e., they were luminance decrements relative to the background). Saccadic suppression was stronger with dark backgrounds than with bright backgrounds. Error bands denote SEM across subjects. (B) Similar results were obtained in condition 3. Flashes over a dark background (when saccades were made from a bright to a dark patch) were associated with stronger suppression than flashes over a bright background (when saccades were made from a dark to a bright patch). (C) Perceptual thresholds (averaged across subjects) for each flash time after saccade onset. Higher values mean stronger saccadic suppression, and error bars denote SEM across the subjects. In both conditions 2 and 3, flashes over a dark background (solid) were associated with much stronger saccadic suppression than flashes over a bright background (dashed). All other conventions are similar to Figure 4.
Figure 5.
 
Stronger saccadic suppression over dark backgrounds, whether with luminance edges or luminance stripes. (A) Average psychometric curves of perceptual detectability from all subjects in condition 2, and for probes occurring 40 ms after saccade onset (i.e., the first time cluster in Figures 1B–D). Solid indicates flashes over a dark background (i.e., saccades starting and ending on a dark patch, but crossing a bright stripe), and dashed indicates flashes over a bright background (i.e., saccades starting and ending on a bright patch, but crossing a dark stripe). In both cases, the probe flashes had negative luminance polarity (i.e., they were luminance decrements relative to the background). Saccadic suppression was stronger with dark backgrounds than with bright backgrounds. Error bands denote SEM across subjects. (B) Similar results were obtained in condition 3. Flashes over a dark background (when saccades were made from a bright to a dark patch) were associated with stronger suppression than flashes over a bright background (when saccades were made from a dark to a bright patch). (C) Perceptual thresholds (averaged across subjects) for each flash time after saccade onset. Higher values mean stronger saccadic suppression, and error bars denote SEM across the subjects. In both conditions 2 and 3, flashes over a dark background (solid) were associated with much stronger saccadic suppression than flashes over a bright background (dashed). All other conventions are similar to Figure 4.
Figure 6.
 
Stronger saccadic suppression for negative polarity flashes over dark backgrounds when compared to positive polarity flashes. Perceptual thresholds across subjects as a function of flash time after saccade onset (error bars denote SEM across subjects). (A) Data from condition 2 (blue) and condition 1 (black) with flashes appearing over a dark background. The saturated colors show results with negative polarity flashes, and the unsaturated colors show results with positive polarity flashes. Consistent with earlier figures, perceptual saccadic suppression was much stronger (higher absolute values of perceptual thresholds) in condition 2 than in condition 1 (compare colored to black curves). Within each condition, saccadic suppression was stronger with negative polarity flashes than positive polarity flashes, especially near the time of peak saccadic suppression (40 ms). (B) With bright backgrounds, the effects of flash polarity were absent (but suppression was still stronger than in condition 1). Note that the y-axis is different from A, because suppression was weaker for flashes over bright backgrounds (see Figure 5). (C, D) Similar results from condition 3, with saccades crossing a luminance edge. The black curves in this case are the same as in A, B. All other conventions are similar to Figures 45.
Figure 6.
 
Stronger saccadic suppression for negative polarity flashes over dark backgrounds when compared to positive polarity flashes. Perceptual thresholds across subjects as a function of flash time after saccade onset (error bars denote SEM across subjects). (A) Data from condition 2 (blue) and condition 1 (black) with flashes appearing over a dark background. The saturated colors show results with negative polarity flashes, and the unsaturated colors show results with positive polarity flashes. Consistent with earlier figures, perceptual saccadic suppression was much stronger (higher absolute values of perceptual thresholds) in condition 2 than in condition 1 (compare colored to black curves). Within each condition, saccadic suppression was stronger with negative polarity flashes than positive polarity flashes, especially near the time of peak saccadic suppression (40 ms). (B) With bright backgrounds, the effects of flash polarity were absent (but suppression was still stronger than in condition 1). Note that the y-axis is different from A, because suppression was weaker for flashes over bright backgrounds (see Figure 5). (C, D) Similar results from condition 3, with saccades crossing a luminance edge. The black curves in this case are the same as in A, B. All other conventions are similar to Figures 45.
Figure 7.
 
Highly similar visual dependencies of perceptual suppression in the absence of saccades. (A) Psychometric curve of perceptual detectability from condition 4, in which a saccade-like displacement of a vertical luminance edge took place (conceptually similar to condition 3 with real saccades). For reference, the black curve shows the psychometric curve from condition 1 (real saccades over a uniform background) at 40 ms. The saccade-like image displacement caused large suppression (a larger rightward shift of the curve), similar to the large suppression seen with real saccades (e.g., Figure 4). Error bands denote SEM across subjects. (B) Time course of perceptual suppression in condition 4. For reference, the time course of saccadic suppression over a uniform background (condition 1) is also shown in black. (C) There was also stronger suppression with (negative polarity) flashes over a dark background than with the same flashes over a bright background, exactly like with real saccades (e.g., Figure 5). The average psychometric curve was shifted more to the right with a dark background (solid). (D) The dependence of perceptual suppression on background luminance was also clear in the whole time course, and this dependence was similar to that seen with real saccades in a similar visual condition of crossing an edge (e.g., Figure 5). For reference, the black curves show the results obtained with real saccades across a uniform dark (solid) or bright (dashed) background (same as in Figure 6).
Figure 7.
 
Highly similar visual dependencies of perceptual suppression in the absence of saccades. (A) Psychometric curve of perceptual detectability from condition 4, in which a saccade-like displacement of a vertical luminance edge took place (conceptually similar to condition 3 with real saccades). For reference, the black curve shows the psychometric curve from condition 1 (real saccades over a uniform background) at 40 ms. The saccade-like image displacement caused large suppression (a larger rightward shift of the curve), similar to the large suppression seen with real saccades (e.g., Figure 4). Error bands denote SEM across subjects. (B) Time course of perceptual suppression in condition 4. For reference, the time course of saccadic suppression over a uniform background (condition 1) is also shown in black. (C) There was also stronger suppression with (negative polarity) flashes over a dark background than with the same flashes over a bright background, exactly like with real saccades (e.g., Figure 5). The average psychometric curve was shifted more to the right with a dark background (solid). (D) The dependence of perceptual suppression on background luminance was also clear in the whole time course, and this dependence was similar to that seen with real saccades in a similar visual condition of crossing an edge (e.g., Figure 5). For reference, the black curves show the results obtained with real saccades across a uniform dark (solid) or bright (dashed) background (same as in Figure 6).
Figure 8.
 
Stronger suppression for negative polarity flashes over dark backgrounds when compared to positive polarity flashes, even in the absence of saccades. (A) Average psychometric curves for negative and positive polarity flashes occurring at 47 ms after image translation onset and appearing over a dark background. Saturated colors show results with negative polarity flashes, and unsaturated colors show results with positive polarity flashes. Error bands denote SEM across subjects. Negative polarity flashes were associated with stronger perceptual suppression than positive polarity flashes. (B) Perceptual thresholds across subjects as a function of flash time after image translation onset in condition 4, comparing negative and positive polarity flashes over a dark background (error bars denote SEM across subjects). For reference, the time course of suppression from condition 1 is also shown in black. Consistent with Figure 7, perceptual suppression with an image translation (condition 4) was much stronger (higher perceptual thresholds) than in condition 1 with real saccades (compare colored to black curves). Moreover, perceptual saccadic suppression was stronger with negative polarity flashes than positive polarity flashes, especially near the time of peak suppression (47 ms). This is identical to the effect that we saw with real saccades in Figure 6. (C) With bright backgrounds, the effects of flash polarity were absent, again consistent with what we observed with real saccades (Figure 6).
Figure 8.
 
Stronger suppression for negative polarity flashes over dark backgrounds when compared to positive polarity flashes, even in the absence of saccades. (A) Average psychometric curves for negative and positive polarity flashes occurring at 47 ms after image translation onset and appearing over a dark background. Saturated colors show results with negative polarity flashes, and unsaturated colors show results with positive polarity flashes. Error bands denote SEM across subjects. Negative polarity flashes were associated with stronger perceptual suppression than positive polarity flashes. (B) Perceptual thresholds across subjects as a function of flash time after image translation onset in condition 4, comparing negative and positive polarity flashes over a dark background (error bars denote SEM across subjects). For reference, the time course of suppression from condition 1 is also shown in black. Consistent with Figure 7, perceptual suppression with an image translation (condition 4) was much stronger (higher perceptual thresholds) than in condition 1 with real saccades (compare colored to black curves). Moreover, perceptual saccadic suppression was stronger with negative polarity flashes than positive polarity flashes, especially near the time of peak suppression (47 ms). This is identical to the effect that we saw with real saccades in Figure 6. (C) With bright backgrounds, the effects of flash polarity were absent, again consistent with what we observed with real saccades (Figure 6).
Figure 9.
 
Weak perceptual suppression with simple luminance steps during fixation. (A) Perceptual contrast thresholds from condition 5 with just luminance steps (colored curves). Each curve shows results for either a positive (dark green) or negative (light green) polarity flash over a dark background, and error bars denote SEM across subjects. For reference, thresholds from condition 1 are shown in black. Peak suppression with a luminance step was as weak as the suppression with real saccades over a uniform background (this was our weakest suppression condition in all experiments; Figures 18). Moreover, during recovery, negative polarity flashes were harder to detect than positive polarity flashes. (B) Similar results for flashes occurring over a bright background. Now, it was positive polarity flashes that were harder to detect than negative polarity flashes during the recovery phase (at times 71 and 106 ms after luminance step occurrence). All other conventions are similar to Figures 45.
Figure 9.
 
Weak perceptual suppression with simple luminance steps during fixation. (A) Perceptual contrast thresholds from condition 5 with just luminance steps (colored curves). Each curve shows results for either a positive (dark green) or negative (light green) polarity flash over a dark background, and error bars denote SEM across subjects. For reference, thresholds from condition 1 are shown in black. Peak suppression with a luminance step was as weak as the suppression with real saccades over a uniform background (this was our weakest suppression condition in all experiments; Figures 18). Moreover, during recovery, negative polarity flashes were harder to detect than positive polarity flashes. (B) Similar results for flashes occurring over a bright background. Now, it was positive polarity flashes that were harder to detect than negative polarity flashes during the recovery phase (at times 71 and 106 ms after luminance step occurrence). All other conventions are similar to Figures 45.
Figure 10.
 
Raw psychometric curves at two flash times from each individual subject in condition 2. (A) Psychometric curves of all subjects with a negative polarity flash at 40 ms and dark backgrounds. (B) The same for the flash at 63 ms. There was strong coherence across subjects in the modulations of perceptual detectability in our experiments.
Figure 10.
 
Raw psychometric curves at two flash times from each individual subject in condition 2. (A) Psychometric curves of all subjects with a negative polarity flash at 40 ms and dark backgrounds. (B) The same for the flash at 63 ms. There was strong coherence across subjects in the modulations of perceptual detectability in our experiments.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×