Our main finding that response times were significantly shorter for a syntactically incongruent target word preceded by an invisible sentential context replicated our previous study. We showed that subsequent to a two-word conscious sentential context, syntactically incongruent words reached consciousness faster under interocular suppression (
Hung & Hsieh, 2015). These findings may first appear inconsistent with the literature. For example, studies have documented that, subsequent to a visible congruent linguistic context, people responded faster to the target word (
O'Seaghdha, 1989;
Seidenberg, Waters, Sanders, & Langer, 1984;
Simpson, Peterson, Casteel, & Burgess, 1989). However, we offer several reasons to provide a theoretical background for our findings. First of all, unconscious processing may be tuned to detect inconsistency, saliency, or anomaly in the stimulus, which is evident in unconscious saliency processing (
Hsieh, Colas, & Kanwisher, 2011;
Zhaoping, 2008). Second, although shorter response times have been observed when responding to a word semantically or syntactically related and temporally close to another invisible, undetected, or unrecognized word (e.g., for subliminal semantic priming, see
Costello et al., 2009;
Dehaene et al., 1998;
Yeh, He, & Cavanagh, 2012); for subliminal syntactic priming, see
Berkovitch & Dehaene, 2019;
Iijima & Sakai, 2014), this word-level unconscious priming effect was less clear when it came to the sentence or phrase level. For example, it was reported that semantically incongruent sentences broke through suppression faster than congruent sentences (
Sklar, Levy, Goldstein, Mandel, Maril, & Hassin, 2012; but see a recent failure on replication,
Rabagliati, Robertson, & Carmel, 2018). One determining factor could be stimulus-driven attention—that is, stimuli that attract attention most (e.g., associated words and inconsistent context) enjoy a reduction of response time or better performance (e.g., for facial attractiveness, see
Hung, Nieh, & Hsieh, 2016;
Nakamura & Kawabata, 2018; for erotic body, see
Jiang, Costello, Fang, Huang, & He, 2006). A systematic study on what exactly attracts our attention unconsciously is required to reach a conclusion. Finally, our results are compatible with other recent empirical findings. For example, online temporal integration of a subliminal syntactic structure can be explained by the early occipital sensitivity to syntactic incongruence (
Dikker, Rabagliati, Farmer, & Pylkkänen, 2010). Meanwhile, it has been shown that readers were less likely to skip syntactically incongruent words while reading (
Brothers & Traxler, 2016). This finding suggests that pre-target-word syntactic context forms grammatical constraints that guide attention during sentence comprehension, which in turn makes syntactic inconsistency salient. Nevertheless, the exact mechanism underlying the early breakthrough or response of incongruency in the current paradigm requires further research.