An ANOVA showed significant main effects in all the illusion cases [Ebbinghaus illusion:
F(2,44) = 27.09,
p < 0.001; orientation contrast illusion:
F(2,44) = 26.88,
p < 0.001; Müller-Lyer illusion:
F(2,44) = 52.45,
p < 0.001]. The differences between the conditions were tested with multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction. The
p values were Bonferroni corrected for an α = 0.05. The Ebbinghaus illusion was significant for all the classic ST, the dynamic DS, DM cases [ST:
t(14) = 7.48,
p < 0.001, DS:
t(14) = 2.89,
p = 0.036, DM:
t(14) = 8.14,
p < 0.001]. Moreover, the illusion strength was significantly stronger in the DM condition than the other two [ST vs. DM:
t(14) = −3.25,
p = 0.017; DS vs DM:
t(14) = −8.55,
p < 0.001], which significantly differed [ST vs. DS:
t(14) = 3.75,
p = 0.007], replicating
Mruczek et al. (2015). In contrast, for the Müller-Lyer illusion, the illusion strength was significant for the classic ST and the dynamic DM cases [ST:
t(14) = 9.06,
p < 0.001; DS:
t(14) = 1.04,
p = 0.948; DM:
t(14) = 4.07,
p = 0.003] and was significantly stronger for ST than in the two dynamic versions [ST vs. DS:
t(14) = 9.37,
p < 0.001; ST vs. DM:
t(14) = 6.08,
p < 0.001], which differed significantly from each other [DS vs. DM:
t(14) = −3.87,
p = 0.005]. Finally for the orientation contrast as well, the illusion strength was significant only for the classic ST case [ST:
t(14) = 9.16,
p < 0.001; DS:
t(14) = −1.32,
p = 0.627; DM:
t(14) = −2.00,
p = 0.195] and was stronger in ST than in the two dynamic versions [ST vs. DS:
t(14) = 5.19,
p < 0.001; ST vs. DM:
t(14) = 7.53,
p < 0.001], which did not differ significantly from each other [DS vs. DM:
t(14) = −0.50,
p = 0.999]. To summarize the effects of the dynamic procedure from above, the Ebbinghaus illusion was significantly increased, whereas the Müller-Lyer and contrast illusions were significantly reduced, and indeed, the contrast illusion no longer differed from 0.