In a first step, we sought to test participant adaptation to the protocol (virtual environment, task and the gaze-contingent protocol), and we expected average visual search times to decrease as a function of trial blocks. We expected vision loss to impact search time, search initiation time, scanning time, verification time,
1 scanpath length, and scanpath ratio, reflecting difficulties in accomplishing the task without central information to analyze regions of interest or peripheral information to identify potential targets to fixate next (
Loschky & McConkie, 2002;
Nuthmann, 2013,
2014;
Nuthmann & Malcolm, 2016). We analyzed scanpath length in addition to scanpath ratio, because we decomposed it into gaze, eye, and head scanpath length in order to study the contribution of head movements to visual search. We expected head movements to contribute by extending the field of fixation (
Rötth, 1925;
Von Noorden & Campos, 2002;
Einh et al., 2007;
Freedman, 2008). We expected search performances to be high overall but to be reduced in the presence of a mask compared to a no-mask condition (
Nuthmann, 2014;
Pereira & Castelhano, 2014;
Cajar et al., 2020). Previous research has shown little effect of central loss of vision on the first phase of visual search (
Nuthmann, 2014;
Nuthmann & Malcolm, 2016). Thus, we expected initiation time not to be affected by central masking.
Nuthmann (2014) and
Nuthmann and Malcolm (2016) reported no significant increase of average scanning times with a central mask. However, choices of mask radius in these two on-screen studies were smaller than in the present study (maximum of 3.5 and 4.1 degrees, respectively). In our case, we expected a mask of 6 degrees of radius to have an equivalent effect as in these previous studies when accounting for the size of the field of view and of objects in the scene. In contrast, we expected average verification times to increase when central vision was masked as participants cannot use central vision to identify targets efficiently (
Nuthmann, 2014;
Nuthmann & Malcolm, 2016;
Cajar et al., 2020). Since the verification phase requires fine object identification with central vision, we expected peripheral masking not to increase average verification times. We expected target refixation rates to increase with central masking but not peripheral masking, due to an increase in return saccades as participants try to analyze regions of interest despite the gaze-contingent mask (
Henderson et al., 1997;
Nuthmann, 2014;
David et al., 2019). Apart from such global effects on search performance, we hypothesized that some objects within a scene would be affected more than others. For instance, objects with a clear anchor relation would be easier to find than targets missing this extra source of guidance (
Boettcher et al., 2018;
Võ et al., 2019). Such anchor objects can be identified peripherally and help guide gaze toward potential locations targets that cluster around the anchor (for a review, see
Võ, 2021). When only central vision is available, finding an anchor object can lead an observer to explore the region around that anchor object preferentially in hope of finding the target. Effects of peripheral loss of vision should be increased when searching for grammatically unconstrained items, apart from verification times and target refixation rates, because the lack of scene grammatical constraint is characterized by impeded guidance in a clustered scene but is less detrimental on identification (
Neider & Zelinsky, 2006;
Mack & Eckstein, 2011). Once an item is found and foveated, its identification is trivial with a peripheral mask, because their size in the field of view will almost always fit within the gaze-contingent mask. In case of central loss of vision, lack of grammatical constraint should hardly affect average initiation and scanning times but should impact target identification.