There are several possible explanations as to why our data do not show a large effect size, whereas some previous studies have shown otherwise. First, we used a modified version of ODI to compute the effect of short-term monocular deprivation as measured in binocular rivalry after previous studies (
Dieter, Sy et al., 2017;
Finn, Baldwin et al., 2019). However, the indices for eye dominance as measured in binocular rivalry do not seem to be identical across studies; these include, but are not limited to, normalized dominance duration (
Kim, Kim et al., 2017), phase duration ratios (
Lunghi, Emir et al., 2015), and deprivation index (
Binda & Lunghi, 2017). In addition, many studies normalize the change in eye balance after short-term patching relative to baseline (i.e., 0), and then conduct data analyses by solely using the difference in data between postpatching and baseline; hence, the relative difference to baseline might seem to be large. As shown in
Figure 8, however, we used our raw data from baseline and postpatching to directly compute the effect size to be more precise. Also, many studies have a smaller sample size than ours (
n = 15), so the reported effect size might have been more prone to a few outliers who displayed an exceptionally large change in sensory eye balance (
Bai, Dong et al., 2017;
Binda, Kurzawski et al., 2018;
Binda & Lunghi, 2017;
Kim, Kim et al., 2017;
Lo Verde, Morrone et al., 2017;
Lunghi, Burr et al., 2011;
Lunghi, Burr et al., 2013;
Lunghi, Galli-Resta et al., 2019;
Ramamurthy & Blaser, 2018;
Wang, McGraw et al., 2020). In addition, although applying a diffuser to one eye is a common method to monocularly deprive its visual input, the methods of monocular deprivation can differ across studies. For example, Bai et al. (
Bai, Dong, He, & Bao, 2017) used pink noise and mean color to deprive the visual input of one eye, whereas Ramamurthy et al. (
Ramamurthy & Blaser, 2018) used kaleidoscopic monocular deprivation.