Although the saccades were infrequent, we analyzed their directions to see if they were truly corrective—namely, to determine whether they aimed back to the midpoint of the physical or perceived paths. To this end, we plotted the saccade amplitude as a function of the distance of its starting point from the physical midpoint. We used data based only on the
x-coordinates (
Figure 5) of the saccades occurring within the analysis windows. In other words, we analyzed horizontal saccadic amplitude as a function of horizontal distance from the physical midpoint. For the no-drift conditions, we expect corrective saccades to head to the right (i.e., have a positive amplitude value) if their starting point was left of the physical midpoint (i.e., if the initial distance had a negative value), correcting the retinal offset of the fovea from the virtual target, and vice versa. For the drift conditions, we expect corrective saccades to be to the right if they started to the left of the virtual midpoint of the perceived locations, and vice versa, independent of their location relative to the physical midpoint. The intercept with the
x-axis of a regression line plotted through these data points reveals the average location of the saccade target, the location where the saccade vector switched from leftward to rightward. As can be seen in
Figure 5, the inferred saccade target (
x-intercept) for the no drift condition was not significantly offset from the physical midpoint; mean offset across participants, 0.31 dva, 95% confidence interval, −0.12 to 0.74 dva,
t(6) = 1.76,
p = .129, Cohen's
d = 0.67, 10.1% of variance explained. In contrast, the
x-intercept was significantly offset from 0 in the double-drift condition: mean offset across participants, 1.02°, 95% confidence interval, 0.62 to 1.42 dva,
t(6) = 6.23,
p < .001, Cohen's
d = 2.35, 58.1% of variance explained. The
x-intercept also differed significantly from the
x-intercept in the no-drift condition: mean difference across participants, 0.71 dva,
t(6) = 2.66,
p = .037, Cohen's
d = 1.01, 20.2% of variance explained. Additionally, we looked at the slopes of these regression lines, because negative slopes would indicate that saccades were corrective. Without drift, the average slope was −2.26 (95%-confidence interval, −3.77 to −0.76), whereas in the drift condition it was −1.29 (95% confidence interval, −1.71 to −0.88). Note that this regression was computed and the
x-intercept calculated individually for each participant. However, in
Figure 5 we plot all saccades from all participants. The inferred saccade target location was consistent with the location of the midpoint between the perceived paths, which suggests that this location rather than the physical midpoint was the target of the catch-up saccades in the double-drift condition. This finding also explains why removing all trials with saccades from the analysis weakened the effect.