Open Access
Article  |   October 2021
Orienting of covert attention by neutral and emotional gaze cues appears to be unaffected by mild to moderate amblyopia
Author Affiliations
  • Amy Chow
    Department of Optometry and Vision Science, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Canada
    ahychow@uwaterloo.ca
  • Yiwei Quan
    Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Canada
    ywquan@edu.uwaterloo.ca
  • Celine Chui
    Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Canada
    on.chui@edu.uwaterloo.ca
  • Roxane J. Itier
    Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Canada
    ritier@uwaterloo.ca
  • Benjamin Thompson
    Department of Optometry and Vision Science, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Canada
    Centre for Eye and Vision Research, 17W Science Park, Hong Kong
    Liggins Institute, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
    ben.thompson@uwaterloo.ca
Journal of Vision October 2021, Vol.21, 5. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.11.5
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Amy Chow, Yiwei Quan, Celine Chui, Roxane J. Itier, Benjamin Thompson; Orienting of covert attention by neutral and emotional gaze cues appears to be unaffected by mild to moderate amblyopia. Journal of Vision 2021;21(11):5. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.11.5.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Amblyopia is a developmental disorder of vision associated with higher-order visual attention deficits. We explored whether amblyopia affects the orienting of covert spatial attention by measuring the magnitude of the gaze cueing effect from emotional faces. Gaze and emotion cues are key components of social attention. Participants with normal vision (n = 30), anisometropic (n = 7) or strabismic/mixed (n = 5) amblyopia performed a cued peripheral target detection task under monocular and binocular viewing conditions. The cue consisted of a centrally presented face with left or right gaze (50% validity to target location) and a fearful, happy, or neutral expression. The magnitude of spatial cueing was computed as the reaction time difference between congruent and incongruent trials for each expression. Fearful facial expressions oriented spatial attention significantly more than happy or neutral expressions. The magnitude of the gaze cueing effect in our cohort of mild-to-moderate amblyopia was comparable to that in normal vision and was not correlated with the severity of amblyopia. There were no statistical group or amblyopia subtype differences for reaction time in any viewing condition. These results place constraints on the range of attentional mechanisms affected by amblyopia and possibly suggest normal covert processing of emotional face stimuli in mild and moderate amblyopia.

Introduction
Amblyopia is a neurodevelopmental disorder of vision caused by an impediment to binocular vision such as anisometropia, strabismus, or visual deprivation that is present during early childhood development. Clinically, amblyopia presents as a unilateral loss of visual acuity and impaired stereopsis associated with chronic interocular suppression of the amblyopic eye (Birch, 2013; Hamm, Black, Dai, & Thompson, 2014; Kiorpes, 2006; Meier & Giaschi, 2017; Wallace, Repka, Lee, Melia, Christiansen, Morse, Sprunger, & American Academy of Pediatric Ophthalmology/Strabismus Preferred Practice Pattern Pediatric Ophthalmology Panel, 2018). More generally, amblyopia affects a broad range of sensory functions in both the amblyopic (Asper, Crewther, & Crewther, 2000a; Asper, Crewther, & Crewther, 2000b; Baker, Meese, & Hess, 2008; Hess, Thompson, & Baker, 2014; Hess & Howell, 1977; Levi, 2020; Levi & Harwerth, 1977; Mullen, Sankeralli, & Hess, 1996; Pardhan & Gilchrist, 1992) and non-amblyopic fellow eye (Birch, Jost, Wang, Kelly, & Giaschi, 2019; Meier & Giaschi, 2017). These sensory deficits impact visuomotor behaviors such as saccadic eye movements (Ciuffreda, Kenyon, & Stark, 1978a; Ciuffreda, Kenyon, & Stark, 1978b; Gambacorta, Ding, McKee, & Levi, 2018; Mackensen, 1958; Niechwiej-Szwedo, Goltz, Chandrakumar, Hirji, & Wong, 2010; Niechwiej-Szwedo, Chandrakumar, Goltz, & Wong, 2012; Perdziak, Witkowska, Gryncewicz, Przekoracka-Krawczyk, & Ober, 2014; Perdziak, Witkowska, Gryncewicz, & Ober, 2016; Perdziak, Gryncewicz, Witkowska, Sawosz, & Ober, 2019; von Noorden, 1961) and hand-eye coordination (Grant, Melmoth, Morgan, & Finlay, 2007; Grant & Conway, 2015; Grant & Moseley, 2011; Melmoth, Finlay, Morgan, & Grant, 2009; Niechwiej-Szwedo, Goltz, Colpa, Chandrakumar, & Wong, 2017; Niechwiej-Szwedo, Colpa, & Wong, 2019). In particular, saccadic and manual response times are significantly delayed for stimuli viewed through the amblyopic eye (Gambacorta et al., 2018; Hamasaki & Flynn, 1981; Levi, Harwerth, & Manny, 1979). Even after visibility is accounted for, an irreducible response latency delay remains in strabismic amblyopia (Gambacorta et al., 2018; Levi et al., 1979; Pianta & Kalloniatis, 1998). 
Amblyopia is associated with reduced fixation stability and it has been proposed that frequent fixational eye movements cause unintended shifts of spatial attention that may contribute to attention deficits in amblyopia (McKee, Levi, Schor, & Movshon, 2016; Verghese, McKee, & Levi, 2019). In amblyopia, fixation stability is poorer for the amblyopic eye because of increased microsaccades and ocular drifts (Chung, Kumar, Li, & Levi, 2015; Ciuffreda, Kenyon, & Stark, 1979; González, Wong, Niechwiej-Szwedo, Tarita-Nistor, & Steinbach, 2012; Kelly, Cheng-Patel, Jost, Wang, & Birch, 2019; Raveendran, Babu, Hess, & Bobier, 2014; Raveendran, Bobier, & Thompson, 2019a; Raveendran, Bobier, & Thompson, 2019b; Shaikh, Otero-Millan, Kumar, & Ghasia, 2016; Subramanian, Jost, & Birch, 2013). Fixation is less stable in strabismic amblyopia than anisometropic amblyopia (Chung et al., 2015; Schor & Hallmark, 1978), with more frequent microsaccades and overall displacement from the locus of fixation (Chung et al., 2015). The presence of microsaccades creates a motor refractory period ranging from 150 to 200 ms that can delay the initiation of subsequent saccades (Chung et al., 2015; Otero-Millan, Troncoso, X. G., Macknik, S. L., Serrano-Pedraza, I., & Martinez-Conde, 2008; Schor & Hallmark, 1978) and delay target detection (McKee et al., 2016; Verghese et al., 2019). Eye movements and visual attention are intricately linked, because attention is often allocated to the locus of visual fixation and overtly shifted in conjunction with eye movements (Goldberg & Wurtz, 1972; Rizzolatti, Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umiltá, 1987). Spatial attention can also be deployed covertly, whereby the locus of attention is shifted without a change in fixation (Beauchamp, Petit, Ellmore, Ingeholm, & Haxby, 2001; Corbetta, Akbudak, Conturo, Snyder, Ollinger, Drury, Linenweber, Petersen, Raichle, Van Essen, & Shulman, 1998; Moore & Fallah, 2001; Nobre, Gitelman, Dias, & Mesulam, 2000). Nevertheless, even on a covert attention task, observers inadvertently make small microsaccades in the direction of the cued location (Engbert & Kliegl, 2003; Hafed & Clark, 2002; although see Horowitz, Fine, Fencsik, Yurgenson, & Wolfe, 2007; Meyberg, Sinn, Engbert, & Sommer, 2017 and McCrackin, Soomal, Patel, & Itier, 2019 with gaze cueing). Target discrimination appears to be better when microsaccades are directed toward the target location (Yuval-Greenberg, Merriam, & Heeger, 2014). As a result, microsaccades may interfere with the orienting of covert attention in amblyopia. 
There remains considerable debate regarding to what extent attentional processing is impaired in amblyopia. Several studies in both humans (Ramesh, Steele, & Kiorpes, 2020; Roberts, Cymerman, Smith, Kiorpes, & Carrasco, 2016) and macaques (Pham, Carrasco, & Kiorpes, 2018) found normal spatial cuing of attention in amblyopia, even demonstrating that valid cueing (congruency between cue and target) alleviated the amblyopic eye contrast sensitivity deficit (Pham et al., 2018). In addition, participants with amblyopia performed normally on a simple visual search task involving a distinctive target feature that readily captured attention (Tsirlin, Colpa, Goltz, & Wong, 2018). Conversely, in a conjunctive visual search task requiring a serial search strategy, participants with amblyopia processed items at a slower rate (with either eye) than controls, suggesting a bottleneck of attentional processing (Tsirlin et al., 2018). Several other psychophysical studies have reported attentional deficits affecting both eyes in amblyopia. For example, when performing a line bisection task with either eye, individuals with amblyopia demonstrated a rightward bias similar to patients with a lesion to the right posterior parietal cortex, an area involved in the orienting of spatial attention (Thiel & Sireteanu, 2009). This effect was more pronounced in participants with strabismic amblyopia than anisometropic amblyopia. In addition, attentional tracking of multiple moving objects performed monocularly revealed an amblyopic eye deficit that extended to the fellow eye under high attentional loads (Ho, Paul, Asirvatham, Cavanagh, Cline, & Giaschi, 2006; Secen, Culham, Ho, & Giaschi, 2011). This tracking deficit could not be attributed to impaired motion perception alone and therefore reflected a visual attention deficit (Ho et al., 2006; Secen et al., 2011). A subsequent study used a dichoptic multiple-object tracking task to assess whether attention was allocated unevenly between the two eyes when both eyes were open. A bias in the allocation of attention toward the fellow eye was observed in strabismic but not anisometropic amblyopia (Chow, Giaschi, & Thompson, 2018). A similar effect has recently been reported for a dichoptic enumeration task whereby amblyopic eyes contributed less to task performance than fellow eyes, and strabismic amblyopia was associated with a larger interocular imbalance than anisometropic amblyopia (Wong-Kee-You Wei, & Hou, 2020). The recruitment of additional attentional resources under high attentional load also appears to be impaired in amblyopia (Farzin & Norcia, 2011). Overall, psychophysical studies indicate that although spatial cueing appears to be intact, higher-order attentional processes may be impaired in amblyopia. 
Many of the attentional deficits documented in amblyopia persist despite prior treatment. Although treatment is generally successful in recovering visual acuity in the amblyopic eye, visual processing in amblyopia remains abnormal (Arden, Barnard, & Mushin, 1974; Birch et al., 2019; Hamm et al., 2014; Meier & Giaschi, 2017; Tao, Wu, Gong, Chen, Mao, Chen, Zhou, & Huang, 2019). In particular, individuals with previous treatment of amblyopia still display attentional deficits that affect both the amblyopic and fellow eyes (Chow et al., 2018; Ho et al., 2006; Hou, Kim, Lai, & Verghese, 2016; Secen et al., 2011; Thiel & Sireteanu, 2009; Wang, Crewther, Liang, Laycock, Yu, Alexander, Crewther, Wang, & Yin, 2017; Wong-Kee-You et al., 2020). An electrophysiological study found that despite past treatment, the modulatory effect of a simple central spatial cue is reduced in primary visual area V1, as well as in higher-order visual areas V4 and MT+ for both the amblyopic and fellow eye in strabismic amblyopia (Hou et al., 2016). Similarly, a generalized reduction of activation across the brain areas comprising the attentional network has been observed in strabismic amblyopia even after surgical treatment to alleviate the amblyogenic factor (Wang et al., 2017). On the other hand, some psychophysical studies in mild and treated populations found no attentional deficit (Ramesh et al., 2020; Roberts et al., 2016). Additionally, a correlation between the depth of attentional deficit and the severity of amblyopia (defined as interocular VA difference) is seldom observed (Chow et al., 2018; Farzin & Norcia, 2011; Ramesh et al., 2020; Roberts et al., 2016; Wong-Kee-You et al., 2020; although see Hou et al., 2016; Popple & Levi, 2008; Tsirlin et al., 2018). This discrepancy merits further investigation. 
In this study, we explored cueing of covert spatial attention with cues that involve higher-order processing. We were interested in the possibility that amblyopic eye attentional deficits would emerge within a cueing task if complex visual processing was required to process the cue. Attention can be engaged by social cognition (Hayward & Ristic, 2015; Itier & Batty, 2009; Nummenmaa & Calder, 2009), so we used a dynamic gaze cueing task (Driver, Davis, Ricciardelli, Kidd, Maxwell, & Baron-Cohen, 1999; Friesen & Kingstone, 1998; Frischen, Bayliss, & Tipper, 2007) that oriented visual attention using gaze directions embedded within emotional faces (McCrackin & Itier, 2018; McCrackin & Itier, 2019). Compared to spatial cueing using arrow cues, the gaze direction of a face is more ecologically valid and reflects social interactions in real-world situations. Following another's gaze is critical for joint attention and inferring the mental states of others (Baron-Cohen, 1995; Stephenson, Edwards, & Bayliss, 2021). This form of social attention, which relies on intact face processing (Burra, Kerzel, & Ramon, 2017), integrates local processing of gaze cues with global processing of a face and its emotional expression and involves a distributed network of thalamic and cortical brain regions (Itier & Batty, 2009; Sabatinelli, Fortune, Li, Siddiqui, Krafft, Oliver, Beck, & Jeffries, 2011). By using a dynamic sequence in which a face morphs from a neutral expression to an affective state, a stronger gaze cueing effect is elicited (for gaze cueing reviews, see Dalmaso, Castelli, & Galfano, 2020; Frischen et al., 2007). Although early studies in amblyopia found poor accuracy for identifying facial expressions during amblyopic eye viewing (Lerner, Pianka, Azmon, Leiba, Stolovitch, Loewenstein, Harel, Hendler, & Malach, 2003), poor performance was also found for inverted faces, suggesting that the deficit lies in featural component processing rather than face configural processing. A follow-up study by the same authors found that reduced activation of extra-striate areas was not face-specific and could be attributed to the reduced visibility of the facial features (Lerner et al., 2006). Individuals with amblyopia also showed no deficits on the Mooney face task, commonly used for assessing face detection and relying on holistic face processing (Cattaneo, Vecchi, Monegato, Pece, Merabet, & Carbon, 2013). These findings indicate that limitations on face perception in anisometropic and strabismic amblyopia may be driven by resolution deficits rather than impaired face processing per se. 
In normal vision, processing of gaze cues occurs spontaneously, improving detection of peripheral targets in the direction of gaze (congruent) as compared to targets that appear on the opposite side (incongruent) (Driver et al., 1999; Friesen, Moore, & Kingstone, 2005; Friesen & Kingstone, 1998; Frischen et al., 2007). Although spatial cueing using salient flashes or arrows may be intact in amblyopia (Pham et al., 2018; Ramesh et al., 2020; Roberts et al., 2016), it is unclear whether social cueing based on gaze and emotional cues is affected. Emotional facial expressions modulate the gaze cueing effect in neurotypical observers, especially when the face reacts with an emotional expression after gaze aversion, akin to someone reacting to what they were seeing (Lassalle & Itier, 2015b). Fearful expressions (which signal nearby danger) orient spatial attention more strongly than neutral expressions (Chen, McCrackin, S. D., Morgan, A., & Itier, 2021; Graham, Kelland Friesen, Fichtenholtz, & LaBar, 2010; Lassalle & Itier, 2013; Lassalle & Itier, 2015a; Lassalle & Itier, 2015b; Mathews, Fox, Yiend, & Calder, 2003; McCrackin & Itier, 2018; McCrackin & Itier, 2019; Neath, Nilsen, Gittsovich, & Itier, 2013; Putman, Hermans, & van Honk, 2006; Tipples, 2006). Happy expressions (which suggest a possible reward) orient attention to a similar degree as neutral faces (Bayless, Glover, M., Taylor, & Itier, 2011; Chen et al., 2021; Graham et al., 2010; Hietanen & Leppänen, 2003; Lassalle & Itier, 2013; Lassalle & Itier, 2015b; Neath et al., 2013; Putman et al., 2006; Tipples, 2006). Recent studies have reported a slightly larger orienting response for happy compared to neutral faces as well, but still of smaller magnitude than the orienting response to fearful faces (McCrackin & Itier, 2018; McCrackin & Itier, 2019). 
In this study, we explored whether amblyopia reduces the extent to which emotional face cues orient covert attention. We recruited participants with mild, moderate, or previously treated amblyopia to ensure that the face images were clearly visible to the amblyopic eye and that the local processing of gaze position required for the gaze cueing effect could occur. If the development of social attention to visual cues is affected by amblyopia, we would expect that the gaze cueing effect of a fearful emotional face would be weaker when viewing with an amblyopic eye compared to the fellow eye and to normal control eyes. We also explored whether the magnitude of the gaze cueing effect differed by amblyopia etiology, because attentional deficits may be more pronounced in strabismic amblyopia. 
Methods
Participants
Thirty participants with normal vision (mean age ± SD: 20.8 ± 2.4 yrs; 22 female) and 12 participants with amblyopia (mean age ± SD: 27 ± 11.1 years; five female) were recruited at the University of Waterloo. We aimed to recruit 30 participants in both groups, but we were not able to reach this number for the amblyopia group during the recruitment period. All participants provided written informed consent, and the study protocol was approved by the institutional ethics committee, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants either received course credit or were remunerated for their time. Individuals were ineligible for the study if they self-reported a history of psychiatric or neurological disorder (including seizures or epilepsy) or a past loss of consciousness longer than five minutes associated with head trauma. Participants also reported no recent use of antidepressant or antipsychotic drugs or medications containing cortisone and no regular or recent use of drugs or alcohol. 
Clinical assessment included visual acuity (using an electronic ETDRS chart), eye alignment (distance and near cover test) and stereoacuity (Randot Preschool Stereotest; Stereo Optical Co. Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). All participants wore their habitual correction as needed. Participants with normal vision had best-corrected visual acuity better than 20/25, with no greater than a 1 logMAR line difference in visual acuity between the eyes, and no history of binocular vision disorders. Amblyopia was defined as a minimum of a 2 logMAR line interocular difference in visual acuity or a 1 logMAR line difference with a history of amblyopia treatment, caused by anisometropia (>1 diopter interocular difference or >1.5 diopters of cylinder in one eye) or strabismus (including history of strabismus surgery), with normal ocular and general health. Clinical details for individuals with amblyopia are summarized in Table 1
Table 1.
 
Clinical details for participants with amblyopia. Notes: M = male, F = female, VA = visual acuity, A = anisometropia, S = strabismus, M = mixed (anisometropia and strabismus), NS = non-strabismic, XP = exophoria, X(T) = intermittent exotropia, RET = right esotropia, ∆ = prism diopters, Dx = diagnosed, y = years, yo = years old, VT = vision therapy (orthoptics or dichoptic binocular amblyopia treatment).
Table 1.
 
Clinical details for participants with amblyopia. Notes: M = male, F = female, VA = visual acuity, A = anisometropia, S = strabismus, M = mixed (anisometropia and strabismus), NS = non-strabismic, XP = exophoria, X(T) = intermittent exotropia, RET = right esotropia, ∆ = prism diopters, Dx = diagnosed, y = years, yo = years old, VT = vision therapy (orthoptics or dichoptic binocular amblyopia treatment).
Apparatus
Participants performed the experiment in a chinrest with head stabilization while wearing an Eyelink II head-mounted eye tracker (250 Hz; SR Research; Mississauga, Canada). Eye tracking data were used only to ensure central fixation. Stimuli were presented using Experiment Builder (SR Research; Mississauga, Canada) on a 27″ ASUS PG278QR LCD monitor (2560 × 1440 resolution, 120-Hz refresh rate; Taipei, Taiwan) from a viewing distance of 50 cm. 
Stimuli
Face stimuli previously used in McCrackin and Itier (2018) were used in this experiment. These faces (four male and four female; identities 02, 03, 06, 09, 20, 22, 24, 27) were selected from the widely available NimStim database1 (Tottenham, Tanaka, Leon, McCarry, Nurse, Hare, Marcus, Westerlund, Casey, & Nelson, 2009) and cropped to remove the hair, ears, and clothing. Each face expressed one of three emotions (fear, neutral and happy) and had averted left or right gaze. Each face was vertically flipped to counterbalance any facial asymmetries. To account for apparent motion of the mouth in happy and fearful expressions, neutral expressions with averted gaze featured tongue protrusion (McCrackin & Itier, 2018). Each face (15.9° wide × 23.1° tall) was presented centrally on a white background with a fixation cross between the nasion and the nose. 
Procedure
For participants with normal vision, the dominant eye was defined as the eye more sensitive to blur in the presence of a +2.00 DS lens placed monocularly over each eye while binocularly observing letters 2 logMAR lines above their best-corrected visual acuity threshold (Pointer, 2012). The dominant eye in participants with amblyopia was defined as the eye with better best-corrected visual acuity. 
First, to control for any effect of amblyopia on the perception of emotional faces, all participants rated valence and intensity for 48 faces (across all emotional expressions and gaze directions). Each face was presented to only the non-dominant/amblyopic eye for 400 ms. Participants rated the valence (1 = very negative to 9 = very positive) and intensity (1 = not intense to 9 = very intense) for each face on a visible Likert scale using keyboard presses. 
Second, to measure attentional cueing with emotional face cues, we used an established spatial cueing task (McCrackin & Itier, 2018). After a brief period of fixation (pseudo-random period of 500–800 ms), a neutral face with direct gaze appeared for 300 ms. Gaze was averted to the left or the right for 100 ms and the expression then changed to fearful, neutral (with tongue protrusion) or happy for 400 ms (Figure 1). After face offset, a target asterisk (1.3° × 1.3°) was presented 20o to the left or right of fixation and participants had up to 500 ms to respond to the location of the target using arrow keys while maintaining central fixation. Reaction time and accuracy were measured. 
Figure 1.
 
Sample trial sequence. Fearful, happy and neutral faces from the NimStim database (Tottenham et al., 2009) were used in the experiment, represented here with schematics.
Figure 1.
 
Sample trial sequence. Fearful, happy and neutral faces from the NimStim database (Tottenham et al., 2009) were used in the experiment, represented here with schematics.
All participants were informed that gaze direction was not predictive of target location but to still attend to the emotional expressions because participants would occasionally be asked to verbally report the most recent emotional expression. Gaze cues were congruent or incongruent with target location and had 50% validity. Each block consisted of 384 trials (64 trials per condition) presented in a randomized order and took 30 minutes to complete. 
All participants completed 3 blocks (viewing conditions: dominant eye, non-dominant eye, both eyes) within one session (1.5 to 2 hours) in a randomized order. Participants were encouraged to take breaks between blocks to avoid fatigue. The task was self-paced because participants completed 16 trials at a time before being presented with a break screen. Upon resumption, eye tracking drift correction was performed before starting the next set of trials. Before formal data collection, 24 practice trials were provided to familiarize participants with the detection task. 
Statistical analyses
Only correct responses within 2.5 standard deviations of the mean reaction time were used to compute average reaction times for each condition (Van Selst & Jolicoeur, 1994). Incorrect trials were excluded because reaction times in the presence of a task error do not reflect an appropriate orientation of attention. The gaze cueing effect was computed as the difference in reaction times between incongruent and congruent trials. 
Statistical analyses were performed with JASP version 0.12.1 (Amsterdam, Netherlands). Accuracy was analyzed with a 3 (eye: dominant eye, non-dominant eye, both eyes) × 2 (group: control, amblyopia) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). Perception of emotional faces was evaluated using a 3 (emotion: fear, neutral, happy) × 2 (group: control, amblyopia) repeated measures ANOVA on valence and intensity ratings. Reaction times were analyzed with an omnibus 3 (eye: dominant eye, non-dominant eye, both eyes) × 2 (congruency: congruent, incongruent) × 3 (emotion: fear, neutral, happy) × 2 (group: control, amblyopia) repeated measures ANOVA. In cases where Mauchly's test of sphericity was significant, the Huynh-Feldt correction was applied. Post-hoc analyses were conducted on significant interactions using Tukey's correction for multiple comparisons. 
Results
A significant main effect of emotion (F2,80 = 310.4, p < 0.001, ω2 = 0.82) was found for valence ratings. There was no main effect of group (F1,40 = 1.8, p > 0.05) nor an emotion × group interaction (F2,80 = 1.6, p > 0.05). Post-hoc t-tests showed that fearful expressions had a significantly lower (negative) valence rating than both neutral (Table 2; mean difference ± SE −1.48 ± 0.18, p < 0.001) and happy (−4.4 ± 0.18, p < 0.001) expressions. Neutral expressions had a significantly lower valence rating than happy expressions (−2.9 ± 0.18, p < 0.001). 
Table 2.
 
Participants’ mean ratings of valence and intensity for each emotional face. Reported as mean rating (standard error of the mean).
Table 2.
 
Participants’ mean ratings of valence and intensity for each emotional face. Reported as mean rating (standard error of the mean).
For intensity ratings, significant main effects of emotion (F1.6,65.2 = 81.3, p < 0.001, ω2 = 0.47) and group (F1,40 = 4.1, p = 0.049, ω2 = 0.04) but no emotion × group interaction (F1.6,65.2 = 0.4, p > 0.05) were found. Participants with amblyopia rated faces to be marginally more intense than participants with normal vision (Table 2; 0.52 ± 0.26, Bonferroni corrected p = 0.049). Fearful and happy faces were perceived as more intense than neutral ones (fearful vs. neutral: 2.2 ± 0.2, p < 0.001; happy vs neutral: −2.2 ± 0.2, p < 0.001), and there was no difference between fearful and happy expressions (0.003 ± 0.2, p > 0.05). 
Overall target detection accuracy was high in both control (mean ± SE; 96.9% ± 0.7%) and amblyopia (97.6% ± 0.3%) groups. No main effects of eye (F2,80 = 0.3, p > 0.05) nor group (F1,40 = 0.6, p > 0.05) nor an eye × group interaction (F2,80 = 1.6, p > 0.05) were present. Error rates were slightly higher in the control group (2.1% ± 0.5%) than the amblyopia group (1.5% ± 0.2%), and the percentage of trials with delayed responses were comparable between the two groups (control: 1.0 ± 0.4%; amblyopia: 0.9% ± 0.2%). After trimming reaction times exceeding 2.5 SD of each individual's reaction time to mitigate the influence of outliers, an average of 95.5% ± 0.2% (controls) and 96.7% ± 0.1% (amblyopia) of the data remained for further reaction time analysis. 
A 3 (eye) × 2 (congruency) × 3 (emotion) × 2 (group) analysis on mean reaction time revealed significant main effects of emotion (F1.6,65.5 = 6.1, p = 0.006, ω2 = 0.0007; see Figure 2A) and eye (F2,80 = 5.2, p = 0.008, ω2 = 0.01; see Figure 2B) but no effect of group (F1,40 = 1.4, p > 0.05). Other significant interactions included eye × group (F2,80 = 4.8, p = 0.011, ω2 = 0.01), emotion × group (F1.6,65.5 = 5.6, p = 0.009, ω2 = 0.0006), and eye × emotion × group (F4.1, 164.6 = 3.2, p = 0.013, ω2 = 0.0004). However, no significant pairwise comparisons emerged after Tukey correction. A significant interaction of congruency × emotion was present (F2,40 = 20.6, p < 0.001, ω2 = 0.002) replicating the established emotional gaze cueing effect (Chen et al., 2021; Graham et al., 2010; Lassalle & Itier, 2013; Lassalle & Itier, 2015a; Lassalle & Itier, 2015b; Mathews et al., 2003; McCrackin & Itier, 2018; McCrackin & Itier, 2019; Neath et al., 2013; Putman et al., 2006; Tipples, 2006). Post-hoc analyses revealed that the gaze cueing effect (i.e., the RT difference between incongruent and congruent trials) was stronger for fearful than neutral faces (main difference ± SE 12.2 ± 2.0 ms, Bonferroni corrected p < 0.001) and for fearful than happy faces (8.6 ± 2.0 ms, Bonferroni corrected p < 0.001); neutral and happy faces did not differ (−3.7 ± 2.0 ms, Bonferroni corrected p > 0.05; see Figure 3). Importantly, the interaction of eye × congruency × emotion × group was not significant (F3.9,157.8 = 1.1, p > 0.05), nor was the congruency × emotion × group interaction (F2.1,82.4 = 0.79, p > 0.05)2, suggesting that the emotional cueing effect was similar across groups and eye conditions.3 
Figure 2.
 
Mean reaction times across emotion (A) and viewing condition (B). (C) Individual reaction time data from the amblyopia group, segregated by amblyopia subtype. Circles (with solid lines) = anisometropic amblyopia; triangles (with dotted lines) = strabismic/mixed amblyopia. DE, dominant/fellow eye; NDE, non-dominant/amblyopic eye; BE, both eyes. Error bars denote standard error.
Figure 2.
 
Mean reaction times across emotion (A) and viewing condition (B). (C) Individual reaction time data from the amblyopia group, segregated by amblyopia subtype. Circles (with solid lines) = anisometropic amblyopia; triangles (with dotted lines) = strabismic/mixed amblyopia. DE, dominant/fellow eye; NDE, non-dominant/amblyopic eye; BE, both eyes. Error bars denote standard error.
Figure 3.
 
Box plot diagram showing emotional gaze cueing effects for fear, neutral and happy expressions for participants with normal vision (n = 30) and amblyopia (n = 12). Box boundaries denote twenty-fifth and seventy-fifth percentiles with the line inside box representing the median. Whiskers represent the full range of data points.
Figure 3.
 
Box plot diagram showing emotional gaze cueing effects for fear, neutral and happy expressions for participants with normal vision (n = 30) and amblyopia (n = 12). Box boundaries denote twenty-fifth and seventy-fifth percentiles with the line inside box representing the median. Whiskers represent the full range of data points.
As an exploratory analysis to discern whether there were any differences among subtypes of amblyopia, we separated the amblyopia group into anisometropic (n = 8) and strabismic/mixed (n = 4) subtypes. A 3 (eye) × 2 (congruency) × 3 (emotion) × 2 (subtype) repeated measures ANOVA showed no main effect of subtype (F1,10 = 0.11, p > 0.05) nor any interaction of subtype with other factors including congruency (F1,10 = 2.3, p > 0.05). Although the congruency × emotion interaction remained significant (F2,20 = 7.8, p = 0.003), it did not interact with amblyopia subtype (F2,20 = 1.47, p > 0.05). As seen in Figure 2C, the strabismic/mixed subtype group did not have slower reaction times (mean ± SE: anisometropia 366.86 ± 19.45 vs. strabismic/mixed 357.79 ± 19.45) and trended toward larger gaze cueing effects (anisometropia 19.80 ± 4.1 vs. strabismic/mixed 28.73 ± 4.1), although the difference was not statistically significant. 
Some participants in the amblyopia group reported previous successful treatment of amblyopia and had an interocular acuity difference less than 0.2 logMAR despite residual functional deficits in stereoacuity. We conducted an independent samples t-test to determine whether the magnitude of the gaze cueing effect was driven by this successfully treated group. We found no significant difference in gaze cueing effects between the successfully treated group (n = 3; mean ± SE 33.6 ± 6.5 ms) and the rest of our sample (n = 9; 21.2 ± 2.9 ms; t10 = −1.98, p = 0.08). Omission of these participants from the analysis did not change our main findings. Neither the eye × congruency × emotion × group interaction (F4,148 = 0.98, p > 0.05) or the congruency × emotion × group interaction (F2,74 = 0.2, p > 0.05) were significant. The magnitude of the gaze cueing effect appears to be independent of interocular acuity differences (ρ = −0.11, p > 0.05; see Figure 4). 
Figure 4.
 
Gaze cueing effect as a function of interocular acuity difference in the amblyopia group across different emotions. Each participant is represented by a shape for each emotional cue condition (open circle = fear, open square = neutral, filled triangle = happy). Black = anisometropic, blue = strabismic/mixed. Shapes are slightly offset to avoid overlapping each other.
Figure 4.
 
Gaze cueing effect as a function of interocular acuity difference in the amblyopia group across different emotions. Each participant is represented by a shape for each emotional cue condition (open circle = fear, open square = neutral, filled triangle = happy). Black = anisometropic, blue = strabismic/mixed. Shapes are slightly offset to avoid overlapping each other.
Previous studies have reported larger gaze cueing effects in females than males for neutral expressions (Alwall, Johansson, & Hansen, 2010; Bayliss, di Pellegrino, & Tipper, 2005; Deaner, Shepherd, & Platt, 2007; Feng, Zheng, Zhang, Song, Luo, Li, & Talhelm, 2011; Hayward & Ristic, 2017; McCrackin & Itier, 2019). However, there is no evidence that this difference in gaze cueing effect between sexes varies across emotional expressions (McCrackin & Itier, 2019). Within our sample of 27 females and 15 males, no effect of gender was evident for the gaze cueing effect based on an independent samples t-test (fear: t40 = 0.75, p > 0.05; neutral: t40 = 0.034, p > 0.05; happy: t40 = 0.21, p > 0.05). 
Discussion
The present study explored whether amblyopia affects the attention orienting effect generated by the gaze direction of emotional face cues. We first verified the emotional valence of the stimuli to ensure that any attentional capture effects were not limited by low level visual deficits. Despite using a complex attentional task requiring integration of gaze and emotional cues from face processing, our results suggest that orienting covert attention using emotional face cues is not affected by amblyopia across all viewing conditions. Specifically, no deficit in spatial cueing was seen under amblyopic eye viewing, and we did not find an attentional imbalance favoring the fellow eye in amblyopia under monocular viewing conditions. Although we constrain our findings to those with treated, mild and moderate amblyopia, we find a comparable (even nominally larger) gaze cueing effect in our amblyopia group compared to the control group (Chen et al., 2021; Graham et al., 2010; Lassalle & Itier, 2015a; Lassalle & Itier, 2015b; McCrackin & Itier, 2018) and determined that this result was not driven by the subset of participants with treated amblyopia. In particular, fearful facial expressions oriented covert attention more strongly than happy and neutral expressions (Chen et al., 2021; Fox, Mathews, Calder, & Yiend, 2007; Graham et al., 2010; Hietanen & Leppänen, 2003; Lassalle & Itier, 2013; Lassalle & Itier, 2015b; McCrackin & Itier, 2018; McCrackin & Itier, 2019; Tipples, 2006). Additionally, we did not find a correlation between the strength of the gaze cuing effect and severity of amblyopia, similar to other previous studies with their attentional measures (Chow et al., 2018; Farzin & Norcia, 2011; Ramesh et al., 2020; Roberts et al., 2016; Wong-Kee-You et al., 2020). 
Regardless of emotion, we found that spatial cueing using gaze cues within face stimuli can orient covert attention in amblyopia. These findings corroborate previous work showing that simple attentional cueing is effective in amblyopia regardless of viewing eye (Pham et al., 2018; Ramesh et al., 2020; Roberts et al., 2016). Because emotional processing augmented the basic cueing effect, perception of emotional faces may be similar among our control and amblyopia groups. Indeed, subjective ratings of emotional valence and intensity did not differ substantially between our groups of participants. We were limited in this study to less severe cases of amblyopia so that participants would be able to resolve individual facial features. Nevertheless, our results are consistent with previous work demonstrating intact global facial processing as long as stimuli can be fully resolved by the amblyopic eye (Cattaneo et al., 2013; Lerner et al., 2006). Our findings of emotion-specific effects suggest holistic face processing may be unaffected by amblyopia in our sample. 
Although we postulate that cortical integration of gaze and emotional cues for the purpose of attention orienting remains intact in mild, moderate, and treated amblyopia, it is also possible that the intact cueing effect is mediated by a different route. A subcortical pathway may expedite face detection and gaze processing, even before detection by cortical routes (Johnson, 2005; Senju & Johnson, 2009; Tamietto & de Gelder, 2010). This pathway involves the superior colliculus, pulvinar and amygdala (Pessoa & Adolphs, 2010). In neurotypical individuals, a stronger functional connection between the pulvinar and amygdala is associated with better recognition of fearful facial expressions (McFadyen, 2019). Neuroimaging of the amygdala in neurotypical individuals reveals residual amygdala activity even when fear-conditioned stimuli are masked (Morris, Ohman, & Dolan, 1999). Through this subcortical route, stimuli continue to be processed despite being perceptually suppressed and this processing occurs in parallel with cortical routes (Morris et al., 1999). A previous report has suggested that motion integration in amblyopia involves intact pulvinar processing and engages a different neural network than individuals without amblyopia (Thompson, Villeneuve, Casanova, & Hess, 2012). These subcortical pathways may mediate the seemingly intact emotional face gaze cueing effect in amblyopia. 
Evidence remains mixed as to whether the severity of any attentional deficit differs by amblyopia etiology. Many of the attentional deficits reported in the literature involve individuals with strabismic amblyopia who often have more severe amblyopia (Chow et al., 2018; Hou et al., 2016; Thiel & Sireteanu, 2009; Wang et al., 2017). Under monocular viewing conditions, spatial bias on a line bisection task was more pronounced in strabismic amblyopia than anisometropic amblyopia (Thiel & Sireteanu, 2009). On the other hand, multiple-object tracking under monocular viewing revealed no difference in performance between anisometropic and strabismic amblyopia (Ho et al., 2006). However, when multiple-object tracking was performed under dichoptic conditions, a tracking deficit was found only for participants with strabismic amblyopia (Chow et al., 2018). Participants with anisometropic amblyopia were equivalent to neurotypical controls (Chow et al., 2018). In another dichoptic attention study, the fellow eye contributed substantially more than the amblyopic eye, with the imbalance greater for strabismic amblyopia than anisometropic amblyopia (Wong-Kee-You et al., 2020). Unfortunately, our limited sample size in this study did not permit us to conduct a subgroup analysis to address this question. In the future, a better understanding of the association between attentional deficits and amblyopia etiology will better inform efforts to recover visual acuity and stereopsis using attention-based amblyopia treatments (Huang, Sun, Luo, Liu, Liu, Mansouri, Wong, Wen, Liu, & Wang, 2014; Li, Ngo, Nguyen, & Levi, 2011; Li, Ngo, & Levi, 2015). 
Previous studies have reported that the size of the gaze cueing effect differs between the sexes for neutral expressions. In particular, females demonstrate a larger gaze cueing effects than males (Bayliss, di Pellegrino, & Tipper, 2005; Deaner et al., 2007; Feng et al., 2011; Hayward & Ristic, 2017; McCrackin & Itier, 2019; Moore & Fallah, 2001). There is no evidence that this difference in gaze cueing effect between sexes varies across emotions (McCrackin & Itier, 2019). Exploratory analysis using an independent samples t-test did not find a statistically significant difference in gaze cueing effect between males and females in our data, although we cannot draw a strong conclusion based on our limited sample size. Even if there was a difference that we did not capture with our small sample size, the distribution of females was higher in the control group than the amblyopia group, whereby the absence of an attentional deficit in the amblyopia group would not be driven by gender. 
The non-dominant eye (NDE) in both groups was exposed to the emotional face stimuli for 12.5% more trials than the dominant eye (DE) as part of the subjective rating procedure at the beginning of the experiment. Although we cannot rule out the presence of a practice effect for the NDE driven by this increased exposure, this is unlikely to be responsible for our results for a number of reasons: (1) The rating procedure did not involve target detection practice to expedite reaction times for the detection of a peripheral stimulus. (2) Cue validity was 50% within the main experiment. Therefore, the effect of prior stimulus exposure would apply equally to both incongruent and congruent trials. (3) Any practice effect would apply to the NDE viewing condition for both groups, and thus our between-group comparison remains valid. Even if it reduced the interocular effect, no monocular difference was evident for the NDE between the groups. (4) We have no reason to believe that any practice effect would remain monocular because face processing involves cortical areas that are binocular. 
Although our main analysis considers the magnitude of attentional orienting effect, we did note the overall increased reaction times for the amblyopia group (despite not being statistically significant). This is likely due to the mild nature of amblyopia among our participants because nine of twelve participants with amblyopia had an amblyopic eye acuity of 0.2 logMAR or better, and reaction time deficits may be correlated with visual acuity (Gambacorta et al., 2018; Hamasaki & Flynn, 1981). Previous studies have found prolonged manual responses when stimuli were viewed with the amblyopic eye as compared to fellow eye or binocular viewing (Gambacorta et al., 2018; Levi et al., 1979; Mackensen, 1958; Pianta & Kalloniatis, 1998; von Noorden, 1961). Increasing stimulus strength to the amblyopic eye can reduce this reaction time difference between the eyes only in anisometropic amblyopia (Gambacorta et al., 2018; Pianta & Kalloniatis, 1998), but an irreducible delay remains for strabismic amblyopia (Gambacorta et al., 2018). Within our small sample of anisometropic and strabismic participants, we did not find any differences among subtypes, because reaction time data from our strabismic amblyopia group fell within the range of the anisometropic amblyopia group (see Figure 2C). Nevertheless, the comparable magnitude of gaze cueing effects for both subtypes suggests that both groups benefit similarly from attentional cueing, although we cannot extrapolate these findings to severe amblyopia. By taking the difference between incongruent and congruent reaction times or a gaze-cueing index as a percentage of overall speed, we are able to exclude any overall reaction time delay inherent to the amblyopia group. 
Conclusions
We explored whether amblyopia impairs the development of social attention by examining the orienting of covert attention by emotional face cues. Our results indicate that spatial cueing with emotional cues is unaffected by mild, moderate, and treated amblyopia under monocular and binocular viewing conditions, and that the magnitude of social cueing is similar to neurotypical controls. Future studies should explore attentional cueing in more severe cases of amblyopia. These results place constraints on the range of attentional mechanisms affected by amblyopia and indicate normal processing of emotional face stimuli in mild and treated amblyopia. A better understanding of the attentional mechanisms in amblyopia may help to accelerate the development of new treatments. 
Acknowledgments
Supported by a NSERC PGS-D Grant (AC; Ottawa, ON, Canada) and NSERC Grants RPIN-05394 and RGPAS-477166 (BT; Ottawa, ON, Canada). 
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Amy Chow. 
Email: amy.chow@uwaterloo.ca. 
Address: 200 Columbia Street West, Waterloo, ON N2L 3G1, Canada. 
Footnotes
1  Development of the MacBrain Face Stimulus Set was overseen by Nim Tottenham and supported by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Early Experience and Brain Development. Please contact Nim Tottenham at tott0006@tc.umn.edu for more information concerning the stimulus set.
Footnotes
2  A power analysis computed using G*Power (v3.1.9.6) post-hoc revealed that to find a significant group difference in the gaze cueing effect for the non-dominant eye with 95% power, we would require a total sample size of 364 participants, indicating the likely absence of an effect even with larger groups.
Footnotes
3  Please note that using a gaze-cueing index as a percentage of overall speed ((RTincongruent – RTcongruent)/[(RTincongruent + RTcongruent)/2] x 100) to account for longer reaction times in the amblyopia group did not change any of the results (approach used in Chen et al., 2021; Pecchinenda & Petrucci, 2016; Ramon, Busigny, & Rossion, 2010).
References
Alwall, N., Johansson, D., & Hansen, S. (2010). The gender difference in gaze-cueing: Associations with empathizing and systemizing. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(7), 729–732, https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2010.06.016.
Arden, G. B., Barnard, W. M., & Mushin, A. S. (1974). Visually evoked responses in amblyopia. The British Journal of Ophthalmology, 58(3), 183–192, https://doi.org/10.1136/bjo.58.3.183.
Asper, L., Crewther, D., & Crewther, S. G. (2000a). Strabismic amblyopia. Part 1. Psychophysics. Clinical & Experimental Optometry, 83(2), 49–58, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1444-0938.2000.tb04892.x.
Asper, L., Crewther, D., & Crewther, S. G. (2000b). Strabismic amblyopia. Part 2. Neural processing. Clinical & Experimental Optometry, 83(4), 200–211, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1444-0938.2000.tb05003.x.
Baker, D. H., Meese, T. S., & Hess, R. F. (2008). Contrast masking in strabismic amblyopia: Attenuation, noise, interocular suppression and binocular summation. Vision Research, 48(15), 1625–1640, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2008.04.017.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bayless, S. J., Glover, M., Taylor, M. J., & Itier, R. J. (2011). Is it in the eyes? Dissociating the role of emotion and perceptual features of emotionally expressive faces in modulating orienting to eye gaze. Visual Cognition, 19(4), 483–510, https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2011.552895.
Bayliss, A. P., di Pellegrino, G., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Sex differences in eye gaze and symbolic cueing of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(4), 631–650, https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980443000124.
Beauchamp, M. S., Petit, L., Ellmore, T. M., Ingeholm, J., & Haxby, J. V. (2001). A Parametric fMRI Study of Overt and Covert Shifts of Visuospatial Attention. NeuroImage, 14(2), 310–321, https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0788.
Birch, E. E. (2013). Amblyopia and binocular vision. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, 33(1), 67–84, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.preteyeres.2012.11.001.
Birch, E. E., Jost, R. M., Wang, Y. Z., Kelly, K. R., & Giaschi, D. E. (2019). Impaired Fellow Eye Motion Perception and Abnormal Binocular Function. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 60(10), 3374–3380, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.19-26885.
Burra, N., Kerzel, D., & Ramon, M. (2017). Gaze-cueing requires intact face processing – Insights from acquired prosopagnosia. Brain and Cognition, 113, 125–132, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2017.01.008.
Cattaneo, Z., Vecchi, T., Monegato, M., Pece, A., Merabet, L. B., & Carbon, C. C. (2013). Strabismic amblyopia affects relational but not featural and Gestalt processing of faces. Vision Research, 80, 19–30, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2013.01.007.
Chen, Z., McCrackin, S. D., Morgan, A., & Itier, R. J. (2021). The Gaze Cueing Effect and Its Enhancement by Facial Expressions Are Impacted by Task Demands: Direct Comparison of Target Localization and Discrimination Tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 618606, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.618606.
Chow, A., Giaschi, D., & Thompson, B. (2018). Dichoptic Attentive Motion Tracking is Biased Toward the Nonamblyopic Eye in Strabismic Amblyopia. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science, 59(11), 4572, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.18-25236.
Chung, S. T. L., Kumar, G., Li, R. W., & Levi, D. M. (2015). Characteristics of fixational eye movements in amblyopia: Limitations on fixation stability and acuity? Vision Research, 114, 87–99, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2015.01.016.
Ciuffreda, K. J., Kenyon, R. V, & Stark, L. (1978a). Processing delays in amblyopic eyes: evidence from saccadic latencies. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics, 55(3), 187–196, https://doi.org/10.1097/00006324-197803000-00008.
Ciuffreda, K. J., Kenyon, R. V, & Stark, L. (1978b). Increased saccadic latencies in amblyopic eyes. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 17(7), 697–702, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/669900.
Ciuffreda, K. J., Kenyon, R. V, & Stark, L. (1979). Fixational eye movements in amblyopia and strabismus. Journal of the American Optometric Association, 50(11), 1251–1258, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/521578.
Corbetta, M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., Snyder, A. Z., Ollinger, J. M., Drury, H. A., Linenweber, M. R., Petersen, S. E., Raichle, M. E., Van Essen, D. C., & Shulman, G. L. (1998). A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron, 21(4), 761–773, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80593-0.
Dalmaso, M., Castelli, L., & Galfano, G. (2020). Social modulators of gaze-mediated orienting of attention: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 27(5), 833–855, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01730-x.
Deaner, R. O., Shepherd, S. V., & Platt, M. L. (2007). Familiarity accentuates gaze cuing in women but not men. Biology Letters, 3(1), 65–68, https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2006.0564.
Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze Perception Triggers Reflexive Visuospatial Orienting. Visual Cognition, 6(5), 509–540, https://doi.org/10.1080/135062899394920.
Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vision Research, 43(9), 1035–1045, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00084-1.
Farzin, F., & Norcia, A. M. (2011). Impaired visual decision-making in individuals with amblyopia. Journal of Vision, 11(14), 6–6, https://doi.org/10.1167/11.14.6.
Feng, Q., Zheng, Y., Zhang, X., Song, Y., Luo, Y., Li, Y., & Talhelm, T. (2011). Gender differences in visual reflexive attention shifting: Evidence from an ERP study. Brain Research, 1401, 59–65, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.041.
Fox, E., Mathews, A., Calder, A. J., & Yiend, J. (2007). Anxiety and Sensitivity to Gaze Direction in Emotionally Expressive Faces. Emotion, 7(3), 478–486, https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.478.
Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(3), 490–495, https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03208827.
Friesen, C. K., Moore, C., & Kingstone, A. (2005). Does gaze direction really trigger a reflexive shift of spatial attention? Brain and Cognition, 57(1), 66–69, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.025.
Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Gaze cueing of attention: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 694–724, https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.694.
Gambacorta, C., Ding, J., McKee, S. P., & Levi, D. M. (2018). Both saccadic and manual responses in the amblyopic eye of strabismics are irreducibly delayed. Journal of Vision, 18(3), 20–20, https://doi.org/10.1167/18.3.20.
Goldberg, M. E., & Wurtz, R. H. (1972). Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. II. Effect of attention on neuronal responses. Journal of Neurophysiology, 35(4), 560–574, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1972.35.4.560.
González, E. G., Wong, A. M. F., Niechwiej-Szwedo, E., Tarita-Nistor, L., & Steinbach, M. J. (2012). Eye position stability in amblyopia and in normal binocular vision. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 53(9), 5386–5394, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.12-9941.
Graham, R., Kelland Friesen, C., Fichtenholtz, H. M., & LaBar, K. S. (2010). Modulation of reflexive orienting to gaze direction by facial expressions. Visual Cognition, 18(3), 331–368, https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280802689281.
Grant, S., & Conway, M. L. (2015). Reach-to-precision grasp deficits in amblyopia: Effects of object contrast and low visibility. Vision Research, 114, 100–110, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2014.11.009.
Grant, S., Melmoth, D. R., Morgan, M. J., & Finlay, A. L. (2007). Prehension deficits in amblyopia. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 48(3), 1139–1148, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.06-0976.
Grant, S., & Moseley, M. J. (2011). Amblyopia and real-world visuomotor tasks. Strabismus, 19(3), 119–128, https://doi.org/10.3109/09273972.2011.600423.
Hafed, Z. M., & Clark, J. J. (2002). Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts. Vision Research, 42(22), 2533–2545, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00263-8.
Hamasaki, D. I., & Flynn, J. T. (1981). Amblyopic eyes have longer reaction times. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 21(6), 846–853, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7309435.
Hamm, L. M., Black, J., Dai, S., & Thompson, B. (2014). Global processing in amblyopia: A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(JUN), 1–21, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00583.
Hayward, D. A., & Ristic, J. (2015). Exposing the cuing task: the case of gaze and arrow cues. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(4), 1088–1104, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-015-0877-6.
Hayward, D. A., & Ristic, J. (2017). Feature and motion-based gaze cuing is linked with reduced social competence. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 44221, https://doi.org/10.1038/srep44221.
Hess, R. F., & Howell, E. R. (1977). The threshold contrast sensitivity function in strabismic amblyopia: Evidence for a two type classification. Vision Research, 17(9), 1049–1055, https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(77)90009-8.
Hess, R. F., Thompson, B., & Baker, D. H. (2014). Binocular vision in amblyopia: Structure, suppression and plasticity. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 34(2), 146–162, https://doi.org/10.1111/opo.12123.
Hietanen, J. K., & Leppänen, J. M. (2003). Does Facial Expression Affect Attention Orienting by Gaze Direction Cues? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(6), 1228–1243, https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.29.6.1228.
Ho, C. S., Paul, P. S., Asirvatham, A., Cavanagh, P., Cline, R., & Giaschi, D. E. (2006). Abnormal spatial selection and tracking in children with amblyopia. Vision Research, 46(19), 3274–3283, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006.03.029.
Horowitz, T. S., Fine, E. M., Fencsik, D. E., Yurgenson, S., & Wolfe, J. M. (2007). Fixational eye movements are not an index of covert attention. Psychological Science, 18(4), 356–363, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01903.x.
Hou, C., Kim, Y., Lai, X. J., & Verghese, P. (2016). Degraded attentional modulation of cortical neural populations in strabismic amblyopia. Journal of Vision, 16(3), 16, https://doi.org/10.1167/16.3.16.
Huang, L., Sun, X., Luo, G., Liu, S., Liu, R., Mansouri, B., Wong, V. W. L., Wen, W., Liu, H., & Wang, A. H. (2014). Interocular Shift of Visual Attention Enhances Stereopsis and Visual Acuities of Anisometropic Amblyopes beyond the Critical Period of Visual Development: A Novel Approach. Journal of Ophthalmology, 2014. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/615213.
Itier, R. J., & Batty, M. (2009). Neural bases of eye and gaze processing: the core of social cognition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(6), 843–863, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.02.004.
Johnson, M. H. (2005). Subcortical face processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), 766–774, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1766.
Kelly, K. R., Cheng-Patel, C. S., Jost, R. M., Wang, Y.-Z., & Birch, E. E. (2019). Fixation instability during binocular viewing in anisometropic and strabismic children. Experimental Eye Research, 183, 29–37, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.exer.2018.07.013.
Kiorpes, L. (2006). Visual Processing in Amblyopia: Animal Studies. Strabismus, 14(1), 3–10, https://doi.org/10.1080/09273970500536193.
Lassalle, A., & Itier, R. J. (2013). Fearful, surprised, happy, and angry facial expressions modulate gaze-oriented attention: Behavioral and ERP evidence. Social Neuroscience, 8(6), 583–600, https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2013.835750.
Lassalle, A., & Itier, R. J. (2015a). Autistic traits influence gaze-oriented attention to happy but not fearful faces. Social Neuroscience, 10(1), 70–88, https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2014.958616.
Lassalle, A., & Itier, R. J. (2015b). Emotional modulation of attention orienting by gaze varies with dynamic cue sequence. Visual Cognition, 23(6), 720–735, https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2015.1083067.
Lerner, Y., Hendler, T., Malach, R., Harel, M., Leiba, H., Stolovitch, C., & Pianka, P. (2006). Selective fovea-related deprived activation in retinotopic and high-order visual cortex of human amblyopes. NeuroImage, 33(1), 169–179, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.026.
Lerner, Y., Pianka, P., Azmon, B., Leiba, H., Stolovitch, C., Loewenstein, A., Harel, M., Hendler, T., & Malach, R. (2003). Area-Specific Amblyopic Effects in Human Occipitotemporal Object Representations. Neuron, 40(5), 1023–1029, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00720-7.
Levi, D. M. (2020). Rethinking amblyopia 2020. Vision Research, 176, 118–129, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2020.07.014.
Levi, D. M., & Harwerth, R. S. (1977). Spatio-temporal interactions in anisometropic and strabismic amblyopia. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 16(1), 90–95, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/832970.
Levi, D. M., Harwerth, R. S., & Manny, R. E. (1979). Suprathreshold spatial frequency detection and binocular interaction in strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 18(7), 714–725, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/447470.
Li, R. W., Ngo, C., Nguyen, J., & Levi, D. M. (2011). Video-game play induces plasticity in the visual system of adults with amblyopia. PLoS Biology, 9(8), e1001135, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001135.
Li, R. W., Ngo, C. V., & Levi, D. M. (2015). Relieving the Attentional Blink in the Amblyopic Brain with Video Games. Scientific Reports, 5, 8483, https://doi.org/10.1038/srep08483.
Mackensen, G. (1958). Reaktionszeitmessungen bei Amblyopie. Albrecht von Graefes Archiv Für Ophthalmologie Vereinigt Mit Archiv Für Augenheilkunde, 159(6), 636–642, https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00685705.
Mathews, A., Fox, E., Yiend, J., & Calder, A. (2003). The face of fear: Effects of eye gaze and emotion on visual attention. Visual Cognition, 10(7), 823–835, https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280344000095.
McCrackin, S. D., & Itier, R. J. (2018). Both fearful and happy expressions interact with gaze direction by 200 ms SOA to speed attention orienting. Visual Cognition, 26(4), 231–252, https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2017.1420118.
McCrackin, S. D., & Itier, R. J. (2019). Individual differences in the emotional modulation of gaze-cuing. Cognition and Emotion, 33(4), 768–800, https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1495618.
McCrackin, S. D., Soomal, S. K., Patel, P., & Itier, R. J. (2019). Spontaneous eye-movements in neutral and emotional gaze-cuing: An eye-tracking investigation. Heliyon. 5(4), e01583, doi:10.1016/j.heliyon.2019.e01583.. [PubMed]
McFadyen, J. (2019). Investigating the Subcortical Route to the Amygdala Across Species and in Disordered Fear Responses. Journal of Experimental Neuroscience, 13, 117906951984644, https://doi.org/10.1177/1179069519846445.
McKee, S. P., Levi, D. M., Schor, C. M., & Movshon, J. A. (2016). Saccadic latency in amblyopia. Journal of Vision, 16(5), 3, https://doi.org/10.1167/16.5.3.
Meier, K., & Giaschi, D. E. (2017). Unilateral amblyopia affects two eyes: Fellow eye deficits in amblyopia. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 58(3), 1779–1800, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.16-20964.
Melmoth, D. R., Finlay, A. L., Morgan, M. J., & Grant, S. (2009). Grasping deficits and adaptations in adults with stereo vision losses. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 50(8), 3711–3720, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.08-3229.
Meyberg, S., Sinn, P., Engbert, R., & Sommer, W. (2017). Revising the link between microsaccades and the spatial cueing of voluntary attention. Vision Research, 133, 47–60, https://doi.org/10.1016/J.VISRES.2017.01.001.
Moore, T., & Fallah, M. (2001). Control of eye movements and spatial attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(3), 1273–1276, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.021549498.
Morris, J. S., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96(4), 1680–1685, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.96.4.1680.
Mullen, K. T., Sankeralli, M. J., & Hess, R. F. (1996). Color and luminance vision in human amblyopia: Shifts in isoluminance, contrast sensitivity losses, and positional deficits. Vision Research, 36(5), 645–653, https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(95)00159-X.
Neath, K., Nilsen, E. S., Gittsovich, K., & Itier, R. J. (2013). Attention orienting by gaze and facial expressions across development. Emotion, 13(3), 397–408, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030463.
Niechwiej-Szwedo, E., Chandrakumar, M., Goltz, H. C., & Wong, A. M. F. (2012). Effects of strabismic amblyopia and strabismus without amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, I: Saccadic eye movements. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 53(12), 7458–7468, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.12-10550.
Niechwiej-Szwedo, E., Colpa, L., & Wong, A. M. F. (2019). Visuomotor behaviour in amblyopia: Deficits and compensatory adaptations. Neural Plasticity, 2019, 16–18, https://doi.org/10.1155/2019/6817839.
Niechwiej-Szwedo, E., Goltz, H. C., Chandrakumar, M., Hirji, Z. A., & Wong, A. M. F. (2010). Effects of anisometropic amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, I: saccadic eye movements. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 51(12), 6348–6354, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.10-5882.
Niechwiej-Szwedo, E., Goltz, H. C., Colpa, L., Chandrakumar, M., & Wong, A. M. F. (2017). Effects of reduced acuity and stereo acuity on saccades and reaching movements in adults with amblyopia and strabismus. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 58(2), 914–921, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.16-20727.
Nobre, A. C., Gitelman, D. R., Dias, E. C., & Mesulam, M. M. (2000). Covert visual spatial orienting and saccades: overlapping neural systems. NeuroImage, 11(3), 210–216, https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0539.
Nummenmaa, L., & Calder, A. J. (2009). Neural mechanisms of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(3), 135–143, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.12.006.
Otero-Millan, J., Troncoso, X. G., Macknik, S. L., Serrano-Pedraza, I., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2008). Saccades and microsaccades during visual fixation, exploration, and search: foundations for a common saccadic generator. Journal of Vision, 8(14), 21.1–21.18, https://doi.org/10.1167/8.14.21.
Pardhan, S., & Gilchrist, J. (1992). Binocular contrast summation and inhibition in amblyopia. The influence of the interocular difference on binocular contrast sensitivity. Documenta Ophthalmologica. Advances in Ophthalmology, 82(3), 239–248, https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00160771.
Pecchinenda, A., & Petrucci, M. (2016). Emotion Unchained: Facial Expression Modulates Gaze Cueing under Cognitive Load. PloS One, 11(12), e0168111, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0168111.
Perdziak, M., Gryncewicz, W., Witkowska, D., Sawosz, P., & Ober, J. (2019). Gap effect and express saccades generation in amblyopia. Journal of Vision, 19(4), 1–17, https://doi.org/10.1167/19.4.17.
Perdziak, M., Witkowska, D., Gryncewicz, W., Przekoracka-Krawczyk, A., & Ober, J. (2014). The amblyopic eye in subjects with anisometropia show increased saccadic latency in the delayed saccade task. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 8(OCT), 77, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2014.00077.
Perdziak, M., Witkowska, D. K., Gryncewicz, W., & Ober, J. K. (2016). Not only amblyopic but also dominant eye in subjects with strabismus show increased saccadic latency. Journal of Vision, 16(10), 1–11, https://doi.org/10.1167/16.10.12.
Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a “low road” to “many roads” of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 11(11), 773–783, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2920.
Pham, A., Carrasco, M., & Kiorpes, L. (2018). Endogenous attention improves perception in amblyopic macaques. Journal of Vision, 18(3), 11–11, https://doi.org/10.1167/18.3.11.
Pianta, M. J., & Kalloniatis, M. (1998). Characteristics of anisometropic suppression: Simple reaction time measurements. Perception and Psychophysics, 60(3), 491–502, https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03206869.
Pointer, J. S. (2012). Sighting versus sensory ocular dominance. Journal of Optometry, 5(2), 52–55, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.optom.2012.03.001.
Popple, A. V, & Levi, D. M. (2008). The attentional blink in amblyopia. Journal of Vision, 8(13):12, 1–9, https://doi.org/10.1167/8.13.12.
Putman, P., Hermans, E., & van Honk, J. (2006). Anxiety meets fear in perception of dynamic expressive gaze. Emotion, 6(1), 94–102, https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.94.
Ramesh, P. V., Steele, M. A., & Kiorpes, L. (2020). Attention in visually typical and amblyopic children. Journal of Vision, 20(3), 11, https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.3.11.
Ramon, M., Busigny, T., & Rossion, B. (2010). Impaired holistic processing of unfamiliar individual faces in acquired prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia, 48(4), 933–944, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.014.
Raveendran, R. N., Babu, R. J., Hess, R. F., & Bobier, W. R. (2014). Transient improvements in fixational stability in strabismic amblyopes following bifoveal fixation and reduced interocular suppression. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 34(2), 214–225, https://doi.org/10.1111/opo.12119.
Raveendran, R. N., Bobier, W. R., & Thompson, B. (2019a). Binocular vision and fixational eye movements. Journal of Vision, 19(4), 9, https://doi.org/10.1167/19.4.9.
Raveendran, R. N., Bobier, W., & Thompson, B. (2019b). Reduced amblyopic eye fixation stability cannot be simulated using retinal-defocus-induced reductions in visual acuity. Vision Research, 154, 14–20, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2018.10.005.
Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umiltá, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25(1A), 31–40, https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(87)90041-8.
Roberts, M., Cymerman, R., Smith, R. T., Kiorpes, L., & Carrasco, M. (2016). Covert spatial attention is functionally intact in amblyopic human adults. Journal of Vision, 16(15), 30, https://doi.org/10.1167/16.15.30.
Sabatinelli, D., Fortune, E. E., Li, Q., Siddiqui, A., Krafft, C., Oliver, W. T., Beck, S., & Jeffries, J. (2011). Emotional perception: Meta-analyses of face and natural scene processing. NeuroImage, 54(3), 2524–2533, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.011.
Schor, C., & Hallmark, W. (1978). Slow control of eye position in strabismic amblyopia. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 17(6), 577–581, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/659082.
Secen, J., Culham, J., Ho, C., & Giaschi, D. E. (2011). Neural correlates of the multiple-object tracking deficit in amblyopia. Vision Research, 51(23–24), 2517–2527, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.10.011.
Senju, A., & Johnson, M. H. (2009). The eye contact effect: mechanisms and development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(3), 127–134, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.009.
Shaikh, A. G., Otero-Millan, J., Kumar, P., & Ghasia, F. F. (2016). Abnormal Fixational Eye Movements in Amblyopia. PloS One, 11(3), e0149953, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149953.
Stephenson, L. J., Edwards, S. G., & Bayliss, A. P. (2021). From Gaze Perception to Social Cognition: The Shared-Attention System. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(3), 553–576, https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620953773.
Subramanian, V., Jost, R. M., & Birch, E. E. (2013). A quantitative study of fixation stability in amblyopia. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 54(3), 1998–2003, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.12-11054.
Tamietto, M., & de Gelder, B. (2010). Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 11(10), 697–709, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2889.
Tao, C., Wu, Y., Gong, L., Chen, S., Mao, Y., Chen, Y., Zhou, J., & Huang, P. C. (2019). Abnormal monocular and dichoptic temporal synchrony in adults with amblyopia. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 60(14), 4858–4864, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.19-27893.
Thiel, A., & Sireteanu, R. (2009). Strabismic amblyopes show a bilateral rightward bias in a line bisection task: Evidence for a visual attention deficit. Vision Research, 49(3), 287–294, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2008.08.005.
Thompson, B., Villeneuve, M. Y., Casanova, C., & Hess, R. F. (2012). Abnormal cortical processing of pattern motion in amblyopia: Evidence from fMRI. NeuroImage, 60(2), 1307–1315, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.078.
Tipples, J. (2006). Fear and fearfulness potentiate automatic orienting to eye gaze. Cognition and Emotion, 20(2), 309–320, https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930500405550.
Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., Marcus, D. J., Westerlund, A., Casey, B. J., & Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168(3), 242–249, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006.
Tsirlin, I., Colpa, L., Goltz, H. C., & Wong, A. M. F. (2018). Visual search deficits in amblyopia. Journal of Vision, 18(4), 17, https://doi.org/10.1167/18.4.17.
Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A Solution to the Effect of Sample Size on Outlier Elimination. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 47(3), 631–650, https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749408401131.
Verghese, P., McKee, S. P., & Levi, D. M. (2019). Attention Deficits in Amblyopia. Current Opinion in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.03.011.
von Noorden, G. K. (1961). Reaction Time in Normal and Amblyopic Eyes. Archives of Ophthalmology, 66(5), 695–701, https://doi.org/10.1001/archopht.1961.00960010697015.
Wallace, D. K., Repka, M. X., Lee, K. A., Melia, M., Christiansen, S. P., Morse, C. L., Sprunger, D. T., & American Academy of Pediatric Ophthalmology/Strabismus Preferred Practice Pattern Pediatric Ophthalmology Panel. (2018). Amblyopia Preferred Practice Pattern. Ophthalmology, 125(1), P105–P142, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ophtha.2017.10.008.
Wang, H., Crewther, S. G., Liang, M., Laycock, R., Yu, T., Alexander, B., Crewther, D. P., Wang, J., & Yin, Z. (2017). Impaired Activation of Visual Attention Network for Motion Salience Is Accompanied by Reduced Functional Connectivity between Frontal Eye Fields and Visual Cortex in Strabismic Amblyopia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11(April), 1–13, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00195.
Wong-Kee-You, A. M. B., Wei, H., & Hou, C. (2020). Feature Counting Under Dichoptic Viewing in Anisometropic and Strabismic Amblyopia. Translational Vision Science & Technology, 9(6), 13, https://doi.org/10.1167/tvst.9.6.13.
Yuval-Greenberg, S., Merriam, E. P., & Heeger, D. J. (2014). Spontaneous microsaccades reflect shifts in covert attention. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 34(41), 13693–13700, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0582-14.2014.
Figure 1.
 
Sample trial sequence. Fearful, happy and neutral faces from the NimStim database (Tottenham et al., 2009) were used in the experiment, represented here with schematics.
Figure 1.
 
Sample trial sequence. Fearful, happy and neutral faces from the NimStim database (Tottenham et al., 2009) were used in the experiment, represented here with schematics.
Figure 2.
 
Mean reaction times across emotion (A) and viewing condition (B). (C) Individual reaction time data from the amblyopia group, segregated by amblyopia subtype. Circles (with solid lines) = anisometropic amblyopia; triangles (with dotted lines) = strabismic/mixed amblyopia. DE, dominant/fellow eye; NDE, non-dominant/amblyopic eye; BE, both eyes. Error bars denote standard error.
Figure 2.
 
Mean reaction times across emotion (A) and viewing condition (B). (C) Individual reaction time data from the amblyopia group, segregated by amblyopia subtype. Circles (with solid lines) = anisometropic amblyopia; triangles (with dotted lines) = strabismic/mixed amblyopia. DE, dominant/fellow eye; NDE, non-dominant/amblyopic eye; BE, both eyes. Error bars denote standard error.
Figure 3.
 
Box plot diagram showing emotional gaze cueing effects for fear, neutral and happy expressions for participants with normal vision (n = 30) and amblyopia (n = 12). Box boundaries denote twenty-fifth and seventy-fifth percentiles with the line inside box representing the median. Whiskers represent the full range of data points.
Figure 3.
 
Box plot diagram showing emotional gaze cueing effects for fear, neutral and happy expressions for participants with normal vision (n = 30) and amblyopia (n = 12). Box boundaries denote twenty-fifth and seventy-fifth percentiles with the line inside box representing the median. Whiskers represent the full range of data points.
Figure 4.
 
Gaze cueing effect as a function of interocular acuity difference in the amblyopia group across different emotions. Each participant is represented by a shape for each emotional cue condition (open circle = fear, open square = neutral, filled triangle = happy). Black = anisometropic, blue = strabismic/mixed. Shapes are slightly offset to avoid overlapping each other.
Figure 4.
 
Gaze cueing effect as a function of interocular acuity difference in the amblyopia group across different emotions. Each participant is represented by a shape for each emotional cue condition (open circle = fear, open square = neutral, filled triangle = happy). Black = anisometropic, blue = strabismic/mixed. Shapes are slightly offset to avoid overlapping each other.
Table 1.
 
Clinical details for participants with amblyopia. Notes: M = male, F = female, VA = visual acuity, A = anisometropia, S = strabismus, M = mixed (anisometropia and strabismus), NS = non-strabismic, XP = exophoria, X(T) = intermittent exotropia, RET = right esotropia, ∆ = prism diopters, Dx = diagnosed, y = years, yo = years old, VT = vision therapy (orthoptics or dichoptic binocular amblyopia treatment).
Table 1.
 
Clinical details for participants with amblyopia. Notes: M = male, F = female, VA = visual acuity, A = anisometropia, S = strabismus, M = mixed (anisometropia and strabismus), NS = non-strabismic, XP = exophoria, X(T) = intermittent exotropia, RET = right esotropia, ∆ = prism diopters, Dx = diagnosed, y = years, yo = years old, VT = vision therapy (orthoptics or dichoptic binocular amblyopia treatment).
Table 2.
 
Participants’ mean ratings of valence and intensity for each emotional face. Reported as mean rating (standard error of the mean).
Table 2.
 
Participants’ mean ratings of valence and intensity for each emotional face. Reported as mean rating (standard error of the mean).
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×