This experiment contained equal numbers of trials with and without a task-irrelevant stimulus event, randomly interleaved, intended to probe stimulus-driven changes in attention. Trials without a task-irrelevant stimulus event followed a standard sequence of three epochs. In the first epoch (“noise”), lasting for 0.6 to 1.2 s, the uniform gray of the intertrial interval was changed to pink visual noise with a root mean square contrast of 3.3%.
In the second epoch (“delay”), two vertically oriented Gabor patches were added to the pink noise, centered on the left or right visual display. The sinusoidal grating of the Gabor patch (87% Michelson contrast) had a spatial frequency of 0.1 cycles per degree and was modulated by a Gaussian envelope with a full width at half maximum of 18° (SD = 7.5°). The phase of the grating was incremented in proportion to the wheel rotation and updated on every monitor refresh so that the sinusoidal pattern displaced on the screen matched the distance that the mouse traveled on the wheel. The Gabor patch on the left drifted leftward, and the Gabor patch on the right drifted rightward, consistent with optic flow. The delay epoch was presented for 1.25 to 2.5 s.
In the third epoch (“change?”), the visual stimuli depended on whether the trial was a change or no-change condition; the two trial types were equally likely, randomly interleaved within a block, and matched for running distance (77–154 cm). On change trials, the Gabor patch on either the left- or right-hand side changed its orientation (left and right changes were equally likely and randomly interleaved within a block). Left Gabor changes rotated clockwise, and right Gabor changes rotated counterclockwise. On no-change trials, neither Gabor patch changed orientation, so that the epoch unfolded as a seamless extension of the previous delay epoch. The average luminance across the visual display in these three epochs was 4 to 8 cd/m2.
Trials with a task-irrelevant stimulus event included the same three epochs above plus two additional epochs: “stimulus event” and “offset delay.” The stimulus event epoch lasted 100 ms and immediately followed the initial delay epoch. For Experiment 1, this stimulus event consisted of a brightening of the Gabor patch, which increased the contrast from 87% to 99% and its overall luminance by 9.4 cd/m2 and is therefore referred to as “flash.” The flash event occurred on half the trials and was equally likely to occur on either the left or right side; hence, the location of the flash was independent of the location of the possible change event.
The flash event was followed by an offset delay epoch that allowed us to match the timing of the change event across trial types; the time from the start of the delay epoch to the end of the offset delay epoch ranged from 1.25 to 2.5 s to match the time of the delay epoch in trials without a flash event. The offset delay epoch contained the same visual stimuli as the second epoch (delay) and lasted 50, 150, 300, 500, or 750 ms, depending on the delays between the flash event and target onset (stimulus-onset asynchrony [SOA]); each SOA was randomly interleaved and equally likely. Finally, the change epoch followed the offset delay epoch, same as in trials without a flash event; in trials with a change, the orientation change was equally likely to occur on either the same side or the opposite side as the previous flash event.
In all trials, the task of the mouse was to lick the spout when he or she detected a change in the orientation of the Gabor patch. Mice had to lick within a 600-ms response window starting 200 ms after the orientation change to score a “hit” and receive a fluid reward. If the mouse failed to lick within this window after an orientation change, the trial was scored as a “miss” and no reward was given but no other penalty was applied. If the mouse licked early during the delay epoch, the trial was aborted and not counted. Premature licks were discouraged with timeouts and possible airpuff penalties (
Krauzlis & Wang, 2018). In trials with a flash event, if the mouse licked after the stimulus but before the response window, the mouse was not punished and the trial progressed forward, following the rule that the stimulus event should be considered behaviorally irrelevant. On no-change trials, if the mouse's first lick in the change epoch fell within the same response window, the trial was scored as a false alarm; if not, the trial was scored as a correct reject. To promote consistent performance, correct reject trials included a safety-net epoch where the Gabor underwent a suprathreshold 30° orientation change and mice could collect a reward by licking in a comparable response window (
Krauzlis & Wang, 2018). Responses in the safety-net epoch were not used in the data analysis.
Experiment 1 was run in blocks of 80 trials. Each block was designed such that there was at least one of each trial type, considering each possible combination of orientation change (change or no-change), change side (left or right), stimulus event (flash or no-flash), flash congruency (congruent or incongruent), and SOA (150, 250, 400, 600, or 850 ms) was present. In other words, within each block, half of the trials (40) had an orientation change and the other half did not. In the half with an orientation change, 20 trials had the change occur on the left Gabor patch and in remaining trials on the right. There was an equal number of trials with and without flash events (10) in each of these 20 trials, five of which were congruent with the side of orientation change and five of which were incongruent. Each of the five congruent and incongruent trials had a different SOA, matching the five SOAs used in the task. The same was true on the other 40 trials without an orientation change, except that they were not divided into change on the left or right; for trials with a flash event, 10 had the flash event on the left and 10 on the right, with two trials for each different SOA used in the task. The order of the trials within a block was randomly interleaved. This block counterbalancing was done to minimize possible behavioral biases related to frequency matching.