The curves that result from measurements of bar-flanked acuity at eccentricities near the fovea appear surprisingly nonhomogeneous, with each curve in
Figure 4 having disparate features, unlike the more regular curves with letter flankers (
Gurnsey et al., 2011). Nevertheless, the clipped line fit, with the appropriate units of threshold elevation versus cortical distance, was able to capture the principal characteristics of the functions remarkably well. Different eccentricities sampled different portions of the canonical curve. Specifically, the “critical spacing” for contour interaction, beyond which no flanker-specific reduction in performance is observed (right-most flat portion), can be described by a constant term in cortical space. Next, for flankers just inside this critical spacing, a sloped portion of the curve is consistent across eccentricities, constituting a canonical form for contour interaction.
However, at even nearer flanker spacings, performance again flattens, yielding a saturated, asymptotic portion. This region does not align in cortical space but instead appears as the stacked colored lines in
Figure 5. However, we found that the outer border of this region occurs at seven to eight times the unflanked threshold bar size at the corresponding eccentricity. Note that this feature of contour interaction is also clearly visible in the results of
Jacobs (1979), who measured acuity for Landolt Cs flanked by bars. The source of the saturation is unclear, although we posit that it is due to a qualitatively different mechanism than the sloping portion that corresponds to crowding, and likely at an earlier stage of processing. One piece of evidence is that the sloped line (including the critical spacing) is consistent across all eccentricities in terms of cortical extent. The critical spacing for the asymptotic portion, on the other hand, can be described as a multiple (7–8) of the unflanked threshold letter acuity. The saturation may mirror the facilitation (or upturn) seen in letter crowding with simple flankers that we (
Coates & Levi, 2014;
Coates et al., 2018) and others (
Siderov et al., 2020) have described in depth. Subjective reports indicated that the closest flanking bars interact with the bars comprising the target Es in a way that provided cues, such as a “double bar” that helps limit target possibilities and lead to better recognition than expected. The flankers may act as
pedestals to improve performance by introducing low-level (contrast) cues that aid in discrimination of the bars of the E (
Coates et al., 2018). While a similar phenomenon with elementary tasks such as gap detection flanked by bars has been reported (
Takahashi, 1968), it is possible that this effect would be abolished with other types of targets or flankers. However,
Siderov et al. (2020) demonstrated the nonmonotonic influence of flanking bars on Sloan letter identification. In the current study, the extent of the saturation zone could be roughly approximated by a constant multiple of the unflanked acuity, suggesting a mechanism more closely tied with resolution acuity, unlike crowding or contour interaction.
We chose to normalize the ordinate of each curve to the corresponding unflanked size threshold at the corresponding eccentricity. In theory, this could be interpreted as normalizing to local units of cortical distance, assuming that threshold unflanked acuity reflects constant cortical scaling (
Duncan & Boynton, 2003). On the other hand, it is clear from
Figure 6 (right panel) that the estimated cortical distance for acuity targets within 5 deg of the fovea is not constant but rather increases with more central targets, in our results as well as prior literature. Further experiments are needed to precisely characterize the relationship of acuity and cortical distance near the fovea.
Flanked size thresholds are related to traditional clinical measures of visual acuity, (
Jacobs, 1979;
Latham & Whitaker, 1996;
Coates et al., 2013;
Chung, 2014;
Song et al., 2014) but differ markedly from the majority of studies of crowding and contour interaction (
Flom et al., 1963;
Toet & Levi, 1992;
Kooi et al., 1994;
Coates et al., 2018;
Marten-Ellis & Bedell, 2021). In these latter studies, an appropriate target size is chosen and target-flanker spacing (but not target size) is varied. Due to the demands of AO psychophysics, it is desirable to limit the duration of testing and make optimal use of each trial spent in the system, motivating the use of adaptive procedures such as QUEST. With the flanked acuity paradigm, there is no need to pretest to determine unflanked performance at each eccentricity. It has been observed that sampling unflanked acuity and only one nominal spacing may be sufficient to define an entire set of results (
Song et al., 2014;
Coates et al., 2013). However, this technique rests heavily on assumptions about the shape of the underlying functions, which has been well characterized for crowding with letters in the periphery (
Song et al., 2014;
Coates et al., 2013). Our characterization of the resultant functions now permits the use of an optimized procedure for bar flankers.
One aspect of the flanked acuity paradigm is that errors can be due to either interference from the flankers or from limitations due to insufficient spatial resolution (
Song et al., 2014). Isolated measurements cannot differentiate between these two influences, although when thresholds are larger than the unflanked size threshold, errors other than blur (e.g., from the flankers) are suggested.
Figure 6 shows that the proportion of flip errors observed differs between these two sources of error, with significantly greater proportion of flip errors as thresholds are elevated from unflanked sizes, despite the same overall performance level in each case. Thus, it would be possible for a psychophysical procedure to use this information to determine error sources, which may be necessary for an adaptive testing procedure. Note, however, that it is unclear if similar effects would be observed with other targets or with other types of flankers.
Dakin et al. (2010) found few 180-deg errors (20%) for Ts flanked by a single flanker composed of a vertical and horizontal line.
Cortical scaling has been employed before to describe the interference on vernier acuity of flanking bars at several locations in the visual field (
Levi et al., 1985), finding an extent of approximately 1 mm.
Tripathy and Levi (1994) expressed their measures of the extent of interactions for T targets and T flankers in terms of cortical distance, estimating 5 to 6 mm, by scaling up estimates from monkeys. More recently,
Pelli (2008) has pointed out how the rule-of-thumb estimate of the critical spacing for crowding of approximately half the eccentricity (
Bouma, 1970) corresponds to about 6 mm on the visual cortex, using the same estimate of
Larsson and Heeger (2006) that we utilized.
Mareschal et al. (2010) found that the influence of Gabor flankers on a Gabor target switched between attraction and repulsion at approximately 0.5 mm.
The efficacy of the cortical scaling model allows the estimation of the exact extent of the interference from the flanking bars. The estimates of 1.3 mm for S1 and 1.5 mm for S2 would correspond to points on crowded proportion correct versus flanker spacing psychometric functions near the unflanked asymptote.
Musilová et al. (2018) estimated contour interaction at various luminances and eccentricities and characterized critical spacing as the distance at which performance drops by
\(\frac{1}{e^3}\), which is a conservative definition of the critical spacing, comparable to our use of the intersection with the asymptotic line.
Figure 8 plots our estimates, along with the empirical measurements reported by
Musilová et al. (2018). Note the two colors indicate different sets of observers and different visual field locations. Lateral interactions in the inferior visual field (red points) would be expected to have a larger extent than on the horizontal meridian (
Greenwood et al., 2017) where we tested. Edge-to-edge distances have been converted to center-to-center distances by adding three fifths of the target letter size as described above.
To contrast our results to crowding with letters, we have plotted the function representing a Bouma ratio (
\(\frac{Critical\; spacing}{Eccentricity}\)) of 0.4 (which corresponds to a critical spacing of around 6 mm). The plots of the “Bouma ratios” (ratio of critical spacing to eccentricity) for our results of flanking with bars are shown in the inset. The functions of both observers asymptote near 0.1 for eccentricities greater than 2 deg, rising sharply near the fovea, as predicted by
Strasburger (2020). It has been known for some time that bars cause less interference than letters (
Flom, 1991) and has been recently demonstrated directly (
Marten-Ellis & Bedell, 2021). Possible reasons for the difference include the inability to confuse a target and flanker, asimilarity between letter targets and bar flankers (
Kooi et al., 1994;
Bernard & Chung, 2011), and the lack of complexity of the bar flankers (
Bernard & Chung, 2011).
Flom (1991) proposed that the more general phenomenon of crowding includes the interference effects from nearby contours (contour interaction), as well as impacts from eye movements and the effects of attention. We believe that the evidence supporting qualitatively different effects in the fovea may reflect experimental limitations, such as inadequate optical correction, which we have overcome with AO. The scale of foveal flanker effects is so small (
Coates et al., 2018) that experimental manipulations to reduce visibility such as lowering the contrast (
Song et al., 2014) or blurring (
Song et al., 2014) may reveal the paradigm-conflated limitations of resolution or overlap masking, rather than flanker interference.
In summary, we have shown that despite the varied nature of results for bar-flanked letter acuity in the fovea and parafovea, expressing results in terms of cortical distance standardizes the curves across eccentricities. The critical distance of 1.3 to 1.5 cortical millimeters (asymptoting at approximately 0.1*E for larger eccentricities) may correspond to the size of canonical cortical processing modules (
Levi et al., 1985) or to sampling by a fixed number of retinal ganglion cells (
Kwon & Liu, 2019). While some aspects of our results may be unique to bar flankers (such as the asymptotic zone with proximal bars), differences between crowding with bars and crowding with letters may simply be quantitative, captured by the coefficients of the crowding region of the canonical curve. Further, the ability of the template clipped line to describe the results suggests that common mechanisms underlie flanked acuity across the visual field, including near the fovea.