Results from Exp. 2b further show that the congruent, attractive integration bias occurs only if the whole stimulus conveys duration information. If only a subset of the dot array marks the interval, the bias does not emerge. This suggests that magnitude integration is driven by the number of dots effectively conveying duration information, and not necessarily by the entire dot array itself. In this case, however, in the absence of an attractive bias, one might have expected an opposite repulsive effect. Indeed, if the blinking subgroup and the rest of the array are treated as two distinct stimuli, we could have observed the same repulsive effect as in Exp. 1a and 2a. To make the subgroup even more salient and different than the rest of the array, we also presented it in red. Still, no effect was observed. This last set of results demonstrates the importance of the spatial congruence of magnitude dimensions for magnitude repulsion to occur. In other words, even if two distinct sets are intermixed with each other, in the absence of a spatial overlap no effect could be observed (i.e., because the dots in the subset and the rest of the array occupied nearby but non-overlapping positions). Additionally, the results of Exp. 2a and 2b rule out an attentional explanation of the effect. Indeed, first, one could argue that the congruent integration effect might emerge as a result of participants actively attending the dot array, while the opposite repulsive effect would arise from the inhibition of the irrelevant distracting numerosity when participants attended the texture interval markers. However, this possibility is less likely because in the “subset” condition of Exp. 2b, due to fact that all experimental conditions were interleaved and the blinking subsets were randomly chosen, it was impossible for the participants to predict which subset of dots to attend. Thus, although participants had still to attend the whole array to perform the task, no effect was observed in this condition, suggesting that attention per se is unlikely to explain the results. Irrespective from this, the inhibition of numerosity information when attending the interval markers in Exp. 1a/2a would predict an abolished, rather than reversed, effect, and a degradation of performance (i.e., lower precision) due to the additional resources needed for inhibition, which was not observed (see
Supplementary Materials). Again, this makes such attentional explanation of the results unlikely. Alternatively, the fact that participants had to potentially divide their attention between the dots and the texture (although not required by the task), might somehow explain the difference in the effect (i.e., underestimation due to less attentional resources dedicated to the interval duration). However, if attention is divided across the two stimuli, it would similarly (or perhaps even more) be divided when the texture did not match the spatial position of the dots. Nevertheless, no effect was observed in such conditions, ruling out a possible influence of divided attention per se. Similarly to the attentional explanation, the results of the subset condition of Exp. 2b also rule out the possibility of a working memory interference. Indeed, one possible explanation for the magnitude integration effect is an interference between magnitude representations concurrently stored in working memory (
Cai et al., 2018). For instance, it has been shown that working memory encoding incorporates the irrelevant dimensions of a target stimulus (
Bocincova & Johnson, 2019), and this may interfere with the task-relevant dimension during decision-making. However, differently from our findings, this type of working memory interference should work for the entire array irrespective of whether only a subset of items blinks to mark the interval.