Data are attached as
Supplementary Material (for both experiments). Results conform to the prediction: Greater internal relative biases were associated with less effective cue combination (
Figure 3). In a multiple linear regression with bimodal precision advantage as the outcome, age was not a significant predictor,
t(92) = −0.06,
p = 0.953, standardized beta = −0.006 (95% confidence interval [CI], −0.204 to 0.193), but relative bias was a significant negative predictor,
t(92) = −2.87,
p = 0.005, standardized beta = −0.287 (95% CI, −0.485 to −0.088). Overall
R2 = 0.0824 for both predictors. Examination of the bimodal precision advantage outcomes suggests that this may have an issue with outliers (
z-scores of −5.2, 2.9, and 5.6; all others less than 2.5), so we will also report non-parametric results. When entering ranks instead of raw values, the age effect was not significant,
t(92) = 0.72,
p = 0.476, standardized beta = 0.071 (95% CI, −0.126 to 0.268), and the bias effect was significant,
t(92) = −3.10,
p = 0.003, standardized beta = −0.308 (95% CI, −0.504 to −0.111). Overall
R2 = 0.0984 for both predictors. This suggests that lower internal relative bias is cross-sectionally correlated with cue combination behavior and that this is not particularly explained by age differences.
We also checked the more basic descriptive results against the previous project with similar methods (
Nardini et al., 2010). The fitted thresholds were also broadly similar (
Figure 4), though slightly higher here. An analysis of overall cue combination also agrees with the previous project. Over the entire sample, precision with both cues was not significantly different from precision with the best single cue,
t(94) = 0.54,
p = 0.588, signed-rank test
p = 0.601. These results are consistent with many previous reports that failed to detect a significant bimodal precision advantage in children at 7 to 10 years old using stimuli and a task like those here (
Nardini et al., 2010) and in other, multisensory settings (
Adams, 2016;
Burr & Gori, 2011;
Dekker et al., 2015;
Gori et al., 2008;
Jovanovic & Drewing, 2014;
Nardini et al., 2008;
Nardini et al., 2013;
Petrini et al., 2014).
In addition, we also carried out a more categorical version of the analysis. The step difference between zero bimodal precision advantage and positive bimodal precision advantage is theoretically meaningful, indicating a difference in algorithm. We classified participants as “combiners” if their estimated precision with both cues was higher than either single cue and as “non-combiners” otherwise (45 combiners vs. 50 non-combiners; mean ages, 9.26 vs. 9.23 years). Combiners had a lower relative bias than non-combiners,
t(93) = −2.14,
p = 0.035, rank-sum test
p = 0.028 (
Figure 5). Note that even in the combiners group, mean relative bias was still well above zero.
To continue the categorical analysis, participants were subject to a median split by relative bias amount (18.6°). Among those with below-median relative bias, precision with both cues was higher than with the best single cue for each participant, t(47) = 2.29, p = 0.026, signed-rank test p = 0.022. This suggests that 7- to 10-year-olds with a lower level of relative bias do combine these two cues to improve their precision at judging slant. Among those with above-median relative bias, precision with both cues was not significantly different from precision with the best single cue, t(46) = −1.55, p = 0.129, signed-rank test p = 0.130. Further, the difference in bimodal precision advantage (precision with both cues minus precision with the best single cue) was significantly different in participants with below-median relative bias versus above-median relative bias, t(93) = 2.72, p = 0.008, rank-sum test p = 0.003. This is an unusual result in the developmental literature, as we found successful precision improvements consistent with cue combination, specifically in those 7 to 10 years old with lower relative bias. This improvement was not seen in those with higher bias, and the measure differed significantly across groups.
In addition to these planned analyses, we also checked for a relation between cue combination behavior and interpupillary distance. This was not significant, r(93) = 0.13, p = 0.218. In other words, we did not find evidence for a direct relation between physical size and cue combination. Future study may be more able to directly address this sort of issue with more extensive (ideally longitudinal) physical growth outcomes.