The central retina in one eye may be affected well before the other, especially at the onset of wet macular degeneration. While this may leave the binocular visual field and good acuity intact, the loss of central vision in one eye impacts stereopsis. Individuals tend to use the high-acuity central retina in the unaffected eye, ignoring the potential for stereopsis in the periphery. This is true for individuals with monocular/non-overlapping scotomata such as M2 and M4, as well as for those with asymmetric scotomata such as B1 and B3. For example, these individuals did not have measurable stereoacuity according to the Randot Stereo Test (worse than 30 arcmins in
Table 1), but our stereoperimetry measurements indicated that they were able to detect coarse disparity steps of 20 arcmins in their periphery. Thus, the periphery clearly has the ability to detect disparity.
Of course, individuals with scotomata in both eyes are also stereoblind in a part of the visual field that has vision loss in either eye. When individuals with binocular scotomata have similar patterns of vision loss in the two eyes, they appear better able to use their peripheral stereopsis, perhaps because the PRLs in the two eyes are in roughly similar locations and are closer to regions with intact retina in both eyes (
Verghese & Ghahghaei, 2020). Although there is evidence that individuals switch PRLs depending on the light level (
Lei & Schuchard, 1997), during reading (
Déruaz, Whatham, Mermoud, & Safran, 2002), and during pursuit (
Shanidze, Fusco, Potapchuk, Heinen, & Verghese, 2015), it is not clear whether individuals with PRLs in different locations in the two eyes switch PRLs from the dominant-eye PRL to a peripheral locus with intact retina in both eyes when they switch from a task that benefits from acuity to one that benefits from stereopsis.
A relevant question is whether the periphery of individuals with macular degeneration has the stereoacuity necessary for tasks of everyday living. As stereoacuity declines with eccentricity (
Fendick & Westheimer, 1983;
McKee, Welch, Taylor, & Bowne, 1990), the residual stereoacuity in maculopathy depends on the eccentricity of healthy retina in corresponding locations in the two eyes. Recall that, whereas binocular acuity is determined by the less impacted eye, stereoacuity requires intact retina loci in both eyes and is thus determined by the eye with the greater extent of vision loss. An inspection of
Table 1 shows that maculopathy participants in our study had PRLs in their non-dominant eye (typically the eye with the larger scotoma) ranging from eccentricities of 1° to over 17°. Larger scotomata are associated with dry macular degeneration, such that the affected retina can extend beyond the macula (which typically extends to about 8° eccentricity) (
Hood, 2015). The smaller scotoma diameters are associated with wet macular degeneration treated with anti-vascular endothelial growth factor injection therapy that typically limit the scotoma diameter to 10°, or an eccentricity of 5°. To determine how stereoacuity falls off with eccentricities up to 15°, we measured stereoacuity in the lower visual field using side-by-side rectangular patches of random dots that scaled with eccentricity (
Ghahghaei, McKee & Verghese, 2016). The goal was to estimate the lower-bound (best) stereoacuity that the participants with MD could achieve at various eccentricities in the periphery, depending on the size of their scotoma. For control participants, stereoacuity thresholds were less than 0.5 arcmins at an eccentricity of 6°, consistent with other studies where the disparity stimuli were scaled with eccentricity (
Fendick & Westheimer, 1983;
McKee et al., 1990), about 1 minute of arc at an eccentricity of 9°, and less than 3 minutes of arc at an eccentricity of 15°. So, in theory, PRLs at eccentricities of 5° to 9° have stereoacuity thresholds around 1 arcmin, which is adequate for many eye–hand coordination tasks such as picking up a cup by its handle or placing pegs in a peg board (
Verghese et al., 2016). Even a PRL at an eccentricity of 15° is capable of detecting a curb drop-off of about 15 cm, which has a disparity of 3 to 10 arcmins depending on the distance from the curb (2–0.7 m for an individual with eyes at a height of 1.65 m and an interocular separation of 6.5°). Thus, the intact periphery in maculopathy could indeed support the coarse stereoacuity required for eye–hand coordination and mobility.