As in
Experiment 2, the size judgment error, and thus the absolute difference between the judged and the presented object size, was taken as the dependent variable. Similarly, the data were again collapsed among positions status (old vs. new). A mixed ANOVA on the judgment errors was run with the within-subjects factors test phase (pre- vs. post-) and position status (old vs. new), as well as the between-subjects factor size change direction (increase vs. decrease).
The ANOVA revealed two significant interaction effects: first, the interaction of change direction × test phase, F(1, 14) = 7.64, p = 0.015, ηG2 = 0.03, and, second, the interaction of position status × test phase, F(1, 14) = 5.05, p = 0.041, ηG2 < 0.01. All other effects were not significant: change direction, F(1, 14) = 0.94, p = 0.349, ηG2 = 0.06; position status, F(1, 14) = 4.57, p = 0.051, ηG2 < 0.01; test phase, F(1, 14) = 0.13, p = 0.720, ηG2 < 0.01; change direction × position status, F(1, 14) = 0.22, p = 0.647, ηG2 < 0.01; or change direction × position status × test phase, F(1, 14) = 0.74, p = 0.405, ηG2 < 0.01.
Furthermore, a Bayesian ANOVA was run (see
Supplementary Tables S5 and
S6). Looking at the inclusion Bayes factors, one can see that the only predictor that shows evidence for its inclusion is the interaction change direction × test phase (BF
incl = 165.33). For all other predictors, there is anecdotal to moderate evidence against including them. Looking at the model comparison, one can see that the model containing exactly this interaction has the highest Bayes factor (BF
10 = 30.93). Due to the principle of marginality, it also includes the respective main effects (test phase + change direction + test phase: change direction). This model is 15 times more likely than the model including the three-way interaction in question.
The significant interaction effect for both the old and the new target positions can be seen in
Figure 6a, although not as distinctly as in the previous experiments. If the size of the object increased in the acquisition phase, then participants perceived the object bigger in the post-test phase than in the pre-test phase. Conversely, they perceived it as smaller in the post-test phase than in the pre-test phase if the size of the object decreased in size in the acquisition phase.
As in the other experiments, a learning index was calculated. This time, the judgment error in the post-test phase was subtracted from the judgment error in the pre-test phase for the group that experienced a size decrease in the acquisition phase. The opposite subtraction was performed for the size increase group. As is depicted in
Figure 6b, the learning index did not differ significantly between the old and new target positions,
t(15) = 0.76,
p = 0.458,
d = 0.19, BF
10 = 0.329. Moreover, the learning index was significantly higher than zero at old positions,
t(15) = 2.50,
p = 0.025,
d = 0.62, BF
10 = 2.637, and new positions,
t(15) = 2.95,
p = 0.010,
d = 0.74, BF
10 = 5.544. Averaged over the medians of all participants, the mean saccade latency was 160.1 ms (
SD = 28.9 ms). On average, the saccade lasted 42.6 ms (
SD = 3.9 ms), and removal of the objects occurred after 21.8 ms (
SD = 0.6 ms).