There was some preprocessing of the coloring. As expected, the luminance of the rendered images was much higher in the highlight regions, leading to dark representations of image regions besides the specular highlights on the display. Thus nonlinear tone mapping was first applied to the original luminance according to the following equations:
\begin{eqnarray*}k = \frac{{\log \frac{1}{{255}}}}{{{L_w}}}\end{eqnarray*}
\begin{eqnarray*}f\left( x \right) = \frac{{{L_{max}}}}{2}(1 - \exp \left( {kx} \right)),\end{eqnarray*}
where
Lw is the luminance of white,
x is the luminance of the original image,
Lmax is the maximum luminance of the display, and
f(x) is the luminance after tone mapping. We used
Lw = 1.5. By applying tone mapping,
f(
x) was found to have values ranging from 0 to
Lmax/2. The tone mapping function is illustrated in
Figure 3.
Supplementary Figure S2 shows the luminance histograms of the object regions in some resultant images. In addition, before the coloring procedure, the chromaticity of all image pixels was metameric to the white point of the display ((
u′,
v′) = (0.188, 0.438)).
The chromaticities used for the coloring procedure were defined on the CIE1976
u′v′ chromaticity diagram. Nine chromaticities were used: an achromatic color and eight chromatic colors. The achromatic color had a
u′v′ chromaticity corresponding to the white point of the display ((
u′,
v′) = (0.188, 0.438)). The eight chromatic colors had chromaticities centered on the achromatic color and in eight color directions at 45° intervals from 0° (
u′ positive direction). The color directions on the
u′v′ chromaticity diagram are shown in
Figure 4. The saturations of chromatic colors (Euclidean distances from the achromatic color) were the same across all eight color directions. The saturation settings are described in the following subsection.
Under both BC and DC coloring conditions, coloring was applied only to the object regions. Subsequently, the colored object regions were embedded in the background image. A background image was created for each object shape and illumination condition by rendering an image (e.g.,
Figure 2 in the area light condition), and the chromaticity was then set at the display's achromatic point. In addition, the object area was made black after rendering. Consequently, the resultant background image contained only the achromatic board under the area light condition, and the floor and walls under the ambient light condition.
In the BC condition, both diffuse and specular reflection components were colored. First, the XYZ values of the diffuse and specular achromatic images were added to create an achromatic image containing both reflection components. The chromaticity of the resultant achromatic image was then manipulated. Based on the luminance Y of the achromatic image, the maximum saturation within the display's color gamut was calculated for the eight color directions. The minimum value of the maximum saturation among the eight color directions was employed as the stimulus saturation and was applied for all color directions. Finally, the chromaticities in the object region were changed to have one of the eight chromaticities.
In the DC condition, only the diffuse reflection component was colored. First, the u′v′ values of the diffuse image were changed to the chromaticity of the eight color directions, as in the BC condition. The colored diffuse and achromatic specular images were then summed in XYZ tristimulus values. The saturation used for coloring the diffuse components was the same across the color directions and was determined in a similar way as that in the BC condition; that is, we searched in a brute-force manner for the maximum saturation of the resultant image that fits within the display's color gamut among the eight color directions.
The coloring differences between the BC and DC conditions appear mainly in the chromaticities of specular highlights.
Figure 5 (a) shows examples of stimulus images of all color directions for the BC and DC conditions, and
Figures 5(b) and (c) show enlarged images with the 180° chromaticity for visibility under the area and ambient light conditions, respectively. The highlights in the BC condition were chromatic, whereas those in the DC condition appeared closer to the achromatic color. This difference provides insight into the main factors contributing to color-induced glossiness enhancement. For instance, a candidate factor for glossiness enhancement is the increment of perceived brightness in the specular highlight regions owing to the H-K effect. If this hypothesis based on the H-K effect is correct, the color glossiness enhancement should be stronger in the BC condition with chromatic specular highlights than in the DC condition with somewhat achromatic highlights. Another candidate factor is the saliency of specular highlights induced by the color contrast between specular highlights and other regions. If this color contrast mainly induces glossiness enhancement, it should be stronger in the DC condition, where only the diffuse component is colored, than in the BC condition, where the diffuse and specular components have the same chromaticity.