A remaining question of course is why observers’ estimations were inaccurate. On a behavioral level, one could argue that humans simply do not assume a constant density of objects in the world. Without that assumption, it would be nonsensical to try to estimate the number of hidden objects based on the number of visible objects. However, it is not obvious why our visual system should not subscribe to this assumption. In most cases, the visible part of the environment should be the best guess for its non-visible parts. In addition, at a lower level, for simple contours and surfaces, the visual system follows the assumption that nothing special happens behind occluders and that information from the surround can be filled-in (de
Weerd, 2006;
Komatsu, 2006). Why does it not follow the same assumption and rules for more complex and irregular arrangements of objects? First, it simply might be an issue of complexity, so that information is not completed at all processing levels, but only for simple low-level features, such as orientation or color. Second, it might be an issue of receptive field size. (Neural) Models of perceptual completion typically assume that neurons with receptive fields at the gap receive lateral input from neurons with receptive fields outside of the gap (
Komatsu, 2006) to complete the missing information in the gap. It is known that receptive field size increases along the visual hierarchy (
Smith, Singh, Williams, & Greenlee, 2001). Completion might fail, if receptive fields are larger than the gaps in information. With respect to these first two points, there is contradictory evidence about the question whether numerosity is a low- or a high-level feature. Some studies found numerosity representations primarily at late processing stages in prefrontal and parietal cortex (
Nieder, Freedman, & Miller, 2002;
Roitman, Brannon, & Platt, 2007), whereas others found numerosity-related activity already in early visual cortex (
DeWind, Park, Woldorff, & Brannon, 2019;
Fornaciai, Brannon, Woldorff, & Park, 2017). Third, although we showed that observers have all the necessary quantities to estimate the number of hidden objects, combining them like in
Equation 4 might be difficult. In particular, calculating the density of the visible objects might not be something that is done intuitively. Because numerosity and density are necessarily intertwined visual features, there has been an intensive debate about whether numerosity is a primary visual attribute and under which circumstances humans use numerosity or texture density as quantitative visual cues (
Burr & Ross, 2008;
Dakin, Tibber, Greenwood, Kingdom, & Morgan, 2011;
Durgin, 2008). Our results clearly show that observers did not use density to solve the task, because using the density of objects in the visible parts of the stimulus would have automatically resulted in accurate choices in experiment 1. Furthermore, the estimation of hidden objects was independent of the size of the occluder and therefore also independent of the density of visible objects, which covary. Hence, our results are consistent with other findings that numerosity can be considered a primary visual attribute (
Cicchini, Anobile, & Burr, 2016) and that it takes precedence over density in our paradigm even when it is not useful for the task. Fourth and finally, it might be an issue of uncertainty. The Bayesian model indicates that the uncertainty about the number of hidden objects might be so large that estimates are completely dominated by the prior (either about the number of all or of hidden objects).