Results: Figure 9 provides an example psychometric function and the Gaussian fit to the data. The acuity threshold is indicated by the vertical dashed line. The resulting acuity values are plotted in
Figure 10. Each panel shows acuity in logMAR units (the logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution in minutes of arc) for the 10 experimental conditions. The three upper panels show the data individually for each participant. The bottom panel shows the average data. A full
\(3\times 2\times 2\) (3 colors, 2 LCA conditions, 2 TCA conditions) ANOVA revealed significant main effects of color (
p = 0.012) and TCA (
p = 0.048), as well as a Color
\({\times }\)TCA interaction (
p = 0.028). The full ANOVA, however, did not find LCA alone to be a significant factor. Because Green was a special condition that was not expected to benefit from LCA or TCA correction, we removed it as a color factor and did a
\(2\times 2\times 2\) ANOVA. In this test, neither TCA nor LCA on their own were significant factors, but the interaction term of TCA
\(\times\) LCA was (
p = 0.036). Collectively, these results suggest that, for the small population studied here, both TCA and LCA need to be corrected to yield a visual benefit. This is generally supported by the data plotted in
Figure 10. To ask more specific questions and do a deeper dive into individual data, we did a series of
t tests on each individual and the aggregated group data. The questions and significance of the outcomes of the tests are shown in
Table 1. The main outcomes are that visual acuity is better with monochromatic than with polychromatic light and that acuity with polychromatic light is only improved by correcting both LCA and TCA. The only subject who benefited from LCA alone was Subject 2, who had very low TCA before and after LCA correction. In summary, we observed a statistically significant improvement in visual acuity in polychromatic light with correction of chromatic aberrations, but the improvement was small.