Eye gaze is a powerful social tool, which serves multiple functions. Gaze enables features in the environment to be processed in depth, while also signaling our intentions to others (
Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, & Jolliffe, 1997;
Risko, Richardson, & Kingstone, 2016). Consequently, it is unsurprising that the ability of another human's gaze to direct attention covertly (without eye movements) and overtly (with eye movements) has been the subject of intense investigation over the past three decades (
Frischen, Bayliss, & Tipper, 2007). Although much is known about the effects of another's gaze direction on these orienting mechanisms separately, few studies have investigated the relationship between covert and overt shifts in attention during gaze-cueing. This is despite intense debate and investigation of this relationship in the general attention literature more broadly (
Casteau & Smith, 2019;
Hunt, Reuther, Hilchey, & Klein, 2019a;
Smith & Schenk, 2012). Therefore, in the current study, we combine computational modeling with a gaze-cueing paradigm to explore the relationship between eye movements and attention directed by social cues. The results shed new light on the relationship between covert and overt orienting during social and non-social cueing.
The gaze-cueing paradigm has been used extensively to investigate the extent to which attention is oriented to the gaze direction of others (
Driver, Davis, Ricciardelli, Kidd, Maxwell, & Baron-Cohen, 1999;
Friesen & Kingstone, 1998). In this variant of a traditional Posner cueing paradigm (
Posner, 1980), participants are presented with a schematic or photograph of a face at fixation. The gaze direction of the cue is then aligned toward the location of an upcoming peripheral target (valid cue condition) or away from it (invalid cue condition). In the classic version of this task, participants are required to maintain central fixation while manual responses are measured. Studies typically report that discrimination and detection of a target are faster and more accurate when preceded by a valid gaze cue relative to an invalid gaze cue. As the participants’ eyes remain at fixation throughout the task, the difference in performance between valid and invalid conditions is typically attributed to a covert shift in spatial attention. Gaze-cues have also been found to have a similar influence on overt shifts in attention. That is, eye movements, or saccades, are initiated faster, and are less likely to land in the wrong position when preceded by a valid gaze-cue relative to an invalid gaze-cue (
Dalmaso, Castelli, & Galfano, 2020;
Itier, Villate, & Ryan, 2007;
Kuhn & Benson, 2007;
Kuhn & Kingstone, 2009;
Ricciardelli, Bricolo, Aglioti, & Chelazzi, 2002).
Although these prior studies have improved our understanding of gaze-cued shifts of covert and overt orienting separately, very little research has experimentally investigated the relationship between these mechanisms during gaze-cueing. Consequently, it remains unclear how these two complementary forms of orienting interact when attention is directed by a social cue. The lack of investigation of this question is surprising for two reasons: (1) there is a growing number of studies that suggest both covert and overt orienting play a role in guiding and facilitating attention during social interactions (
Kuhn, Tatler, & Cole, 2009;
Kuhn, Tatler, Findlay, & Cole, 2008;
Kuhn, Teszka, Tenaw, & Kingstone, 2016;
Laidlaw, Rothwell, & Kingstone, 2016); and (2) the relationship between spatial attention and eye movements has been the subject of intense investigation within the general attention literature more broadly for over four decades (
Klein, 1980;
Posner, 1980;
Rizzolatti, Riggio, Dascola, & Umilta, 1987).
One particularly pervasive question concerns the extent to which these two types of orienting are obligatorily coupled or dissociable (
Casteau & Smith, 2019;
Hunt et al., 2019a;
Smith & Schenk, 2012). Although early studies suggested that spatial cues could not shift the locus of attention away from the goal of an upcoming eye movement (
Hoffman & Subramaniam, 1995;
Rizzolatti et al., 1987), more recent research has called into question this conclusion (
Casteau & Smith, 2018;
Castet, Jeanjean, Montagnini, Laugier, & Masson, 2006;
Montagnini & Castet, 2007;
Parker, Heathcote, & Finkbeiner, 2020a;
Parker, Heathcote, & Finkbeiner, 2020b;
Parker, Heathcote, & Finkbeiner, 2021;
Smith & Schenk, 2012). There is now growing acceptance within the general attention literature that each type of orienting is, at least partly, dissociable and likely mediated by independent underlying mechanisms (
Casteau & Smith, 2019;
Hunt et al., 2019a;
Klein, 2020;
Li, Pan, & Carrasco, 2021).
Recent research on social attention in ecologically valid scenarios suggests that spatial attention and eye movements may both play a role during social interactions (
Kuhn et al., 2008;
Kuhn et al., 2009;
Laidlaw et al., 2016).
Laidlaw and colleagues (2016), for example, found that people tended to use covert attention to monitor their social environment to determine whether subsequent eye contact was appropriate. On the other hand, Kuhn and colleagues (
2008;
Kuhn et al., 2009;
Kuhn et al., 2016), monitored the covert and overt attention of observers viewing live and recorded magic tricks. The authors reported that although overt attention could be modulated by knowledge of the trick, covert attention could not. When taken together, these studies suggest that covert and overt orienting in social environments play complementary yet distinct roles. In addition,
Morgan, Ball, and Smith (2014) supported and extended this conclusion using an eye abduction paradigm. The eye abduction paradigm involves artificially restricting eye movements in healthy participants by rotating the visual display to an angle of 40 degrees from one eye (
Craighero, Nascimben, & Fadiga, 2004). By virtue of this rotation, it is thought that eye movements cannot be prepared or executed toward the restricted hemifield. The authors reported no cueing effects in the restricted hemifield for peripheral or arrow cues, however, the gaze-cueing effect remained intact. These results led the authors to suggest gaze-cueing did not depend upon oculomotor preparation, and that socially cued attention was likely mediated by a unique underlying mechanism to that associated with non-socially cued attention.
Whereas
Morgan and colleagues (2014) suggest that there may be a dissociation between covert and overt orienting to social information, this study suffers from two limitations, which makes firm conclusions difficult to make. First, the experimental design used by
Morgan and colleagues (2014) involved restricting eye movements, which means it remains to be seen if covert and overt orienting can independently contribute to performance in the same gaze-cueing task when participant's eye movements are not artificially restricted. Second,
Morgan and colleagues (2014) analyzed reaction time (RT) and accuracy separately and made comparisons between task blocks. In such designs, it is not possible to disentangle the effects of response caution, which can vary across blocked task conditions, from true differences in the attention effect on performance (
Donkin, Averell, Brown, & Heathcote, 2009). Imagine, for example, that participants displayed more cautious responding to gaze cues in one block, such that responses were slower but more accurate, and less cautious responding to peripheral cues in a separate block, such that decisions were faster but less accurate. It is difficult to disentangle in a separate analysis of accuracy and RT whether these differences in performance are due to differences in response caution, or due to the operation of the cue. Consequently, it is unclear to what extent the relationship between covertly oriented spatial attention and saccade preparation varies by cue type.
The current study addresses these limitations in two ways. First, we used an experimental design – the saccadic dual task – that can independently manipulate eye movements and gaze-cues in order to investigate the relationship between covert shifts in spatial attention and overt shifts in attention that are associated with preparing an eye movement (
Castet et al., 2006;
Deubel, 2008;
Hoffman & Subramaniam, 1995;
Montagnini & Castet, 2007). Second, we analyzed the data using an evidence accumulation computational model that allows comparisons to be made across both blocked task conditions and between subjects’ conditions by providing a principled way of combining accuracy and the distribution of RTs for correct and error responses (
Brown & Heathcote, 2008;
Donkin, Brown, & Heathcote, 2011; see
Figure 1). Importantly for our purposes, this parameterization allows differences in response caution to be quantified and separated from differences due to task difficulty. The benefit of doing so is that we can draw inferences about orienting without contamination from strategic differences that can occur across blocked task conditions.
Combining the saccadic dual task with evidence accumulation modeling has recently been used to study the relationship between covert and overt orienting in non-social settings (
Parker et al., 2020a;
Parker et al., 2020b;
Parker et al., 2021). Across these studies, the authors reported a separate and dissociable contribution of both covert orienting and saccade preparation, the magnitude of which differed in size. The influence of preparing an eye movement on performance was greater than that of a sudden onset peripheral cue. Furthermore, when the spatial acuity demands of the perceptual discrimination task were increased the authors reported that the magnitude of the saccade congruency effect selectively increased (
Parker et al., 2021). When taken together these results led the authors to conclude that covertly oriented spatial attention and eye movement preparation made a quantitatively and qualitatively distinct contribution to perception and that covert and overt orienting were likely mediated by distinct underlying mechanisms.
The aim of the present study was to build on the work by Parker and colleagues (
2020a;
Parker et al., 2020b;
Parker et al., 2021) and use evidence accumulation modelling to explore the relationship between covertly oriented spatial attention and oculomotor preparation during social and non-social cueing tasks. Across two experiments, a cueing paradigm was used to direct spatial attention covertly while eye movements were prepared or remained at fixation. The spatial cue could be a social gaze-cue or a non-social arrow cue (
Kuhn & Kingstone, 2009;
Morgan et al., 2014). We took as our main dependent measure the drift rate parameter of the Linear Ballistic Accumulator (
Brown & Heathcote, 2008), as this parameter can capture both the quality and amount of information accumulating about a decision (
Lewandowsky & Oberauer, 2018). Given these characteristics drift rates provide a measure by which to compare the contributions of covert and overt orienting across tasks and cue types that is not contaminated by differences that can occur across blocked or between subject conditions. We preregistered three hypotheses about the relationship between each type of orienting during social cueing. First, if covertly oriented spatial attention and saccade preparation during gaze-cueing are dissociable (
Kuhn et al., 2008;
Kuhn et al., 2016;
Laidlaw et al., 2016;
Morgan et al., 2014), there will be a distinct contribution of both saccade congruency and cue validity to performance on dual-task trials. That is, we predict an effect of both saccade congruency and cue validity on performance. Furthermore, consistent with previous studies (
Parker et al., 2020a;
Parker et al., 2020b;
Parker et al., 2021), we predict that the magnitude of these effects will differ such that the magnitude of the saccade congruency effect will be greater than the cue validity effect. Second, if both types of orienting are mediated by independent underlying mechanisms, the magnitude of the gaze cueing effect (difference between a valid and invalid gaze cued trial in drift rate) will not vary as a function of eye movement preparation or fixation (
Parker et al., 2020a;
Parker et al., 2020b;
Parker et al., 2021). Third, if similar underlying mechanisms mediate the relationship between each type of orienting during social and non-social cueing, we predict that the same pattern of dissociation between covertly oriented spatial attention and saccade preparation evident for gaze-cues will also be present for nonpredictive centrally presented arrow cues. That is, for both cue types, there will be a main effect of saccade congruency, cue validity, and no interaction between the two. We have no specific predictions about how the magnitude of the main effects may differ by cue type.