There are some limitations to our study. First, eye movements are likely to occur for longer SOAs in this study, which could potentially invalidate visual crowding. Our study showed that the benefits of peripheral cues on crowding relief increased with the length of SOA. However, the effect at lengthy SOAs could have been caused by eye movements to the cued location. Although we made an effort to minimize eye movements by emphasizing to each participant the importance of keeping their eyes on the central fixation both through the instruction and verbally, we could not be certain that all participants would adhere to our instruction. However, this does not mean that the cues did not contribute to relieving crowding. Eye movements to an unexpected location take 150 to 200 ms to initiate (
Carpenter, 1988;
Findlay, 1997). Given that the stimuli in our study were presented in a more peripheral location than earlier studies (e.g.,
Findlay, 1997), it should take even longer to make a saccade to the target even if eye movements did occur. Thus for exogenous attention, eye movements could not have occurred at short SOAs of 100 ms and 175 ms, yet it had significant effects even at that times. Endogenous attention takes about 300 ms to be deployed (e.g.,
Carrasco, 2011), but the present study showed that endogenous attention had significant effects even at the shorter SOAs (e.g., 275 ms) even though eye movements are unlikely to have occurred at that short time. Furthermore, eye movements caused by following endogenous cues may have been minimal in
Experiment 3, even at lengthy SOAs. This is because participants tend to ignore endogenous cues that are not predictive of the target's location (
Giordano et al., 2009;
Kinchla, 1980;
Sperling & Melchner, 1978). Therefore, although we cannot rule out the possibility of eye movements, the pattern of results could not, at least not entirely, be explained by them. If eye movements were well controlled, we would anticipate that the cueing effects would be the same at shorter SOAs but smaller at longer SOAs than what our results revealed. That is, the results would follow a similar pattern but with a lower curve plateau. Future research may use eye-trackers to monitor eye movements to examine if the pattern of results shown in our study could be replicated. Second, because of the drop of the uncrowded condition in
Experiments 2 and
3, we cannot determine how the two types of attention alleviate crowding effect differently. In
Experiments 2 and
3, a new independent variable—cue validity—was included, so the number of total trials would tremendously increase if other designs were the same as
Experiment 1. To prevent potential fatigue effect, we had to simplify the experimental design. Considering that the main purpose of the current study was to compare how exogenous and endogenous attention differ in relieving the discrimination of crowded targets, we removed the uncrowded condition. However, without the uncrowded condition as a baseline, we cannot know how the two types of attention differentially alleviate crowding effect (the difference between the performance on the crowded and uncrowded displays).