We found a nonrandom style embedding of a stimulus set where we held subject matter constant while using the LMDS approach. This suggests that even when high-level background information and mid-level content information have been removed by presenting a single object (apple) only, participants can still consistently perceive style differences. Apparently, there are object properties that make these judgments possible. This will be investigated further in
Experiment 2 by assessing object-related attributes like smoothness and glossiness.
The dimensionality of the MDS analysis was based on 16 landmarks. As we mentioned in
Results, it showed relatively low stress values for dimensions higher than 2 and there was no obvious elbow shape. So, additional criteria were needed. One of these criteria came from the fit of the nonlandmarks in the style space. For the majority of the nonlandmarks (27 out of 32), the data fitted better in the 3D embedding, which made us decide to continue our analysis with the 3D embedding, although stress levels suggested the 2D embedding to be already sufficient.
We fitted the creation year to the 3D embedding, and Dimension 2 resulted in a substantial correlation,
r = 0.69 (
Figure 6A). In addition, looking at the positions of the 48 cut-outs in
Figure 5, the Dim1–Dim2 plane, a rotational pattern can be discerned, as demonstrated in
Figure 6B. This rotational component can also be associated with creation year and yielded a somewhat higher correlation,
r = 0.70 (
Figure 6C). Interestingly, the half-hidden red circle in the third quadrant in
Figure 6B represents a modern painting from 2019 amid a set of much older paintings. If we consider this painting as a continuation of the modern cluster from the second quadrant, in other words, if we add 360 degrees to the same data point in
Figure 6C (the single point in the top left corner), the rotational correlation will even increase to
r = 0.78. This rotational pattern is particularly interesting because a similar pattern was found by
Elgammal et al. (2018), even though their embedding resulted from computational methods and very different experimental parameters. They used paintings of varying subject matter analyzed by a PCA on a CNN layer resulting from training on style labels, while we reached the embedding using human similarity judgment data. As our study and
Elgammal et al. (2018) are so different, our finding strengthens the possibility that a cyclical pattern is present in the history of European art during the past six centuries.
Looking at the embedding, some other observations can be made. Along Dimension 1, there appears to be a transition of brushstroke coarseness, from fine brushstrokes on the left to coarse brushstrokes on the right side. Brushstroke coarseness can be one of the possible features describing the embedding. As
Figure 6B suggests, the least coarse brushstrokes belong to the modern paintings, while the coarsest ones belong to the impressionists’ paintings from the 19th century. This trend in brushstroke coarseness can be one of the possible features describing the embedding and could have been used by participants as a way to differentiate styles. Another observation is a color gradient in the Dim1–Dim2 plane, from green apples on the top left, to yellow and red apples at the bottom. This gradient suggests that color could also have been used to differentiate styles. These two suggestions will be investigated in
Experiment 2.
In summary, while the results clearly show a robust style space, we have yet to analyze it further. As we tentatively concluded, there appears to be a trend in Dimension 1 that relates to brushstroke coarseness and a trend in Dimension 2 related to hue, which might imply that Dimension 3 could be associated with color saturation and/or brightness. To quantify these latent trends, we conducted a second experiment where we used both perceptual attribute ratings and color measurements.