In this study, we measured SD with a technique designed to study confidence, where participants first chose which of two noisy stimuli they preferred to judge, then made a forced-choice judgment on the orientation of the chosen stimulus. Stimuli with the same orientation as the previous stimulus in that position were chosen more frequently than those of a different orientation, and, more importantly, choosing the same orientation led to better accuracy. The advantage in accuracy for congruent trials persisted for trials up to five-back and accumulated over trials.
SD typically results in a bias in perception, revealed by systematic reproduction errors or other means. Although this has shown to result in more efficient perception, largely by reducing response variance (
Cicchini, Arrighi, Cecchetti, Giusti, & Burr, 2012;
Cicchini et al., 2014;
Cicchini, D'Errico, & Burr, 2022;
Jazayeri & Shadlen, 2010), the question as to whether there are benefits in two-alternative, forced-choice paradigms remains open. With standard two-alternative, forced-choice paradigms, serial dependence, which should lead to a response bias, would not increase sensitivity. If the stimuli are truly random, the bias will help on half of the trials but hinder the other half, resulting in no net gain. However, the current study design showed that continuity of orientation can activate automatic and pre-decisional mechanisms, affecting the confidence-based decisions and also improving the accuracy. These results are in line with evidence that SD acts at the level of sensory mechanisms (
Cicchini et al., 2014;
Cicchini et al., 2017;
Cicchini et al., 2018;
Fischer & Whitney, 2014) rather than decisional processes (
Fritsche et al., 2017). Participants never explicitly chose the stimulus of the same orientation; rather, they chose up or down based on which stimulus they believed had the stronger signal (
Barthelmé & Mamassian, 2009). Yet, this choice led to a small preference in choosing the continuous stimulus. The continuous stimuli were also judged more accurately than the discontinuous stimuli. Although the choice of the continuous stimulus was very close to 50% (about 51%), when it was chosen there was a significant improvement in accuracy, by about 2.4%. This suggests that, even though repetition of orientation leads to only a small increase in choice, when the condition is chosen its orientation is seen more accurately. The most probable explanation for the increase in accuracy is that serial dependence boosts signal strength rather than merely biasing perceptual decisions.
The complex paradigm allowed us to look for other types of SD, such as for the position chosen (irrelevant for the task). Here, the results were quite different. There was a very strong bias to choose the previously selected position, by about 10%. However, most of this bias was also presented for contingency with future stimuli and in the shuffled dataset (∼5%), suggesting that it was not driven by SD. There was also an apparent advantage in accuracy, but, again, this advantage disappeared when compared with shuffled results, except for the contingency on the future. The results are probably explained by individual biases, driven by higher sensitivity of superior or inferior visual fields. The biases were strong, up to 83% and 55% on average. The main message from this analysis is that there are many artifacts that can cause the impression of serial dependence, and these artifacts can be stronger than the real effects. In our paradigm, the task was orientation judgment, and orientation was completely independent of position. Despite the strong artifact related to position stickiness, repetition of orientation reliably affected both stimulus choice and response accuracy. This suggests that the effects of serial dependence, both on choice and accuracy, are robust.
Studies in serial dependence typically measure biases induced by successive stimuli differing by small amounts in a key dimension (often orientation). This study used a technique very similar to that usually employed in serial dependence studies, except that it did not measure bias in perceived orientation but rather what may be considered to be a bias in saliency, leading to increased choice and increased accuracy. Nevertheless, we can be quite confident that the bias in choice and the improvement in accuracy are associated with serial dependence rather than other local after-effects such as adaptation. Our stimuli were very similar to those typically used for serial dependence studies: low spatial frequency gratings of moderate contrasts, often noise masked (
Cicchini et al., 2017;
Cicchini et al., 2018;
Cicchini et al., in press;
Collins, 2020;
Fischer & Whitney, 2014;
Murai & Whitney, 2021). Adaptation paradigms typically use long presentations, well over a second, whereas our presentation was 100 ms. Presentation time is a strong predictor of whether the serial effects will be assimilative or repulsive (
Cicchini et al., in press;
Yoshimoto, Uchida-Ota, & Takeuchi, 2014).
Our study shows that, during a serial dependence paradigm, the sensory representation of the previously presented orientation is effectively boosted. In this respect, the results resemble those of perceptual priming, where repetition of a feature (often color) improves performance, measured as accuracy or reaction times (
Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1994). That a typical serial dependence paradigm also leads to improvements suggests that the two processes share common neural mechanisms; however, this suggestion is not supported by other evidence.
Galluzzi et al. (2022) reported evidence suggesting that the two are quite distinct, in that there is very little correlation between many important aspects. This does not exclude the possibility that the two processes share some neural circuitry, but current evidence would suggest that they are not identical. Clearly, more research is needed to understand the exact interplay.
To sum up, we present a new paradigm for investigating SD, as it provides a clean technique to dissociate the effects of real SD from artifacts.