After completion of the exposure phase, subjects performed a binocular rivalry test to assess the influence of triplet sequence learning on perceptual selection. Each trial began with sequential presentation of the first two gratings from one of the four triplets from the exposure phase. This was immediately followed by presentation of a pair of rivalrous gratings of orthogonal orientations, one of which always matched the third grating in the triplet (the predicted orientation) (
Figure 1B). Subjects reported which of the two orientations they perceived. Based on previous studies (
Denison et al., 2011;
Denison et al., 2016;
Piazza et al., 2018), we expected that the effects of prediction would be largest at the beginning of each rivalry presentation, when predictive context is strongest. We therefore analyzed the initial response following the presentation of the rivalrous pair.
In order to investigate the effects of prediction on initial perceptual selection, we first tested for an overall effect of prediction, comparing the proportion of trials in which the predicted grating was initially perceived during the rivalry test to chance (0.5) across all subjects in all groups. We found that across all subjects, initial perception during rivalry was greater for the predicted orientation than for the unpredicted orientation (mean = 0.52,
SEM = 0.0071,
t(79) = 3.28,
p = 0.002, Cohen's
d = 0.69).
Figure 3 shows the difference between the proportion of trials in which the predicted versus unpredicted orientation was initially perceived.
The magnitude of this effect (the predicted orientation was initially perceived on 52% of trials) is very similar to other studies of the effects of prediction on perceptual selection in binocular rivalry. The unpredicted image was selected on 52.6% of binocular rivalry trials following statistical learning of sequences of natural images (
Denison et al., 2016), and the predicted stimulus was selected on 52.5% of binocular rivalry trials following statistical learning of pairs of auditory and visual stimuli (
Piazza et al., 2018). Natural scenes containing embedded incongruous objects (which presumably violated subjects’ priors about the content of natural scenes) were selected 53% of the time compared to natural scenes that were congruous (
Mudrik, Deouell, and Lamy 2011).
Next, we investigated if the length of exposure influenced the effects of prediction on perceptual selection and whether these effects of prediction were maintained throughout the rivalry test. We previously found that the predictive effects of statistical learning on perceptual selection in binocular rivalry can be fleeting, occurring in the first half of the rivalry test trials but dissipating by the second half (
Denison et al., 2016). We conducted a two-way mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA) with two factors: four levels of group (four experimental groups with different durations of exposure) and two levels of run (each run consisting of the first half and second half of the rivalry test respectively). Effect sizes were quantified with partial eta-squared values. We examined the influence of these two factors on the likelihood of an observer perceiving the expected orientation. We found no significant effects of either group (
F(3, 76) = 0.46,
p = 0.71, η
2p = 0.019) or run (
F(1, 79) = 0.24,
p = 0.63, η
2p = 0.033) and no significant interaction between group and run (
F(3, 76) = 1.30,
p = 0.28, η
2p = 0.051).
We also assessed the effects of prediction on the mean latency and duration of the initial response. The duration of each trial was 5 s, allowing measurement of a complete first response on the majority of trials: 71% of trials had an initial response that terminated before the end of the trial. Unlike the analysis of the identity of the initial response (expected vs. unexpected orientation) presented above, initial response latencies and durations can be analyzed separately for expected versus unexpected orientation responses.
For initial latency and duration, we investigated the effects of prediction on initial perceptual selection with a three-way ANOVA that had four levels of group (four experimental groups with different durations of exposure), two levels of run (each run consisting of the first half and second half of the rivalry test, respectively), and two levels of prediction (reported grating was either expected or unexpected).
We found a significant effect of prediction on the duration of the initial response (
Figure 4; main effect of prediction,
F(1, 79) = 19.25,
p < 0.05, η
2p = 0.22), such that the initial duration of the predicted grating (mean = 2,390 ms,
SEM = 49 ms) was significantly shorter than the initial duration of the unpredicted grating (mean = 2,486 ms,
SEM = 49 ms) (Cohen's
d = 0.16). The effects of run and group were not significant (run:
F(1, 79) = 0.66,
p = 0.42, η
2p = 0.006; group:
F(3, 76) = 1.81,
p = 0.15, η
2p = 0.075). Additionally, there were no significant interactions between any of the factors (interaction between prediction and run:
F(1, 76) = 0.37,
p = 0.54, η
2p = 0.009; prediction and group:
F(3, 76) = 1.84,
p = 0.91, η
2p = 0.005; group and run:
F(3, 76) = 1.31,
p = 0.28, η
2p = 0.045; prediction/group/run:
F(3, 76) = 1.58,
p = 0.2, η
2p = 0.068). The relatively short rivalry stimulus presentation duration that we employed did not allow for analysis of responses that occurred after termination of the initial percept.
We additionally found a significant effect of prediction on the latency of the initial response (
Figure 5; main effect of prediction
F(1, 79) = 9.90,
p = 0.002, η
2p = 0.096), such that the initial latency of the predicted grating (mean = 996 ms,
SEM = 21 ms) was significantly shorter than the initial latency of the unpredicted grating (mean = 1,026 ms,
SEM = 22 ms) (Cohen's
d = 0.11). There was also a main effect of run (
F(1, 79) = 4.38,
p = 0.04, η
2p = 0.083), such that the rivalry responses in the first run of trials had a longer latency (mean = 1,028 ms,
SEM = 21 ms) than those in the second run of trials (mean = 994 ms,
SEM = 22 ms). The effect of group was not significant (
F(3, 76) = 0.039,
p = 1.0, η
2p = 0.002). There were no significant interactions between any of the factors (interaction between prediction and run:
F(1, 79) = 1.75,
p = 0.19, η
2p = 0.021; prediction and group:
F(3, 76) = 0.35,
p = 0.79, η
2p = 0.003; group and run:
F(3, 76) = 0.85,
p = 0.47, η
2p = 0.031; prediction/group/run:
F(3, 76) = 0.848,
p = 0.2, η
2p = 0.035). In conclusion, the predicted orientation was more likely to be initially selected, and both the mean latency and mean duration of these initial responses were shorter for the predicted orientation.