Participants were seated in a quiet, dimly lit room. The participant's head was positioned on a chin rest with a forehead stabilizer at 130cm from the projection screen that subtended 60° by 34° of visual angle (dva). Stimuli were displayed with a PROPixx projector (VPixx Technologies, Saint-Bruno-de-Montarville, QC, Canada) at 120 Hz. The experiment was programmed and presented with MATLAB using Psychtoolbox (
Brainard, 1997) and Eyelink toolbox (
Cornelissen, Peters, & Palmer, 2002) and was run on an Apple computer. The right eye was monitored using an Eyelink 1000 Plus desktop mount (SR Research, Kanata, ON, Canada) at 1000 Hz.
All stimuli were presented against a mid-gray background. A black fixation point (0.25 dva in diameter) was always presented in the middle of the screen. The stimulus consisted of an annulus (7.5 dva inner radius, 10 dva outer radius) divided into four alternating light and dark sectors. The contrast of the annulus varied from trial to trial and could take one of the five values: 5%, 10%, 20%, 40%, 80%. The annulus rotated at a speed of 135°/s and reversed direction every 90° (80 frames, 666 ms). The polarity of the annulus and the starting direction of motion were counterbalanced within each condition. On every second reversal (two reversals per cycle), motion stopped for 50 ms (six frames) and a green ellipse (2.5° wide and 4° long) with a black target on it was presented on the top sector edge within the annulus. The timing of the reversal was jittered from trial to trial so that the position of the sector edge and its flashed target at the reversal point was varied over ±23.5° of rotation. There were five conditions depending on the shape of the target: filled rectangle, outline rectangle, filled ellipse, outline ellipse, and T-shape.
Based on pilot observations of the various distortions for these stimuli, we generated parametric shape changes that might cancel these distortions (
Figure 2, see demonstration movies at
https://cavlab.net/Demos/ShapeDistortion/DemoAll5). The following distortions were noted: the square appeared to expand in width, the circle expanded to become egg-shaped blunted at leading edge (in the direction of the motion after the reversal), and the vertical stem of the T shifted relative to the top in the direction of the motion after the reversal. The flashed shape has no motion itself but is displaced as if it were in motion with the background after the reversal (
Cavanagh & Anstis, 2013). The “leading edge” is then the edge of the test shape that is farthest from the background contour in the direction of the motion after the reversal. We exaggerated these distortions to generate one end of the adjustment scale and then mirrored these to create a full range of adjustments that should at some point restore the stimuli to the canonical shapes: square, circle, and T. The reference shape was in the middle of the adjustment range and was separated from each end of the range by 40 steps. The illusion end of the shapes was given a score of −1 (left column in
Figure 2), the reference shape in the middle, 0 (middle column
Figure 2), and the shapes with the maximum reversed distortions, a value of +1 (right column,
Figure 2).
Contours were always 0.2 dva thick. Rectangles were 1.5 dva high and had adjustable width (from 0.5 dva to 2.5 dva). Elliptical shapes were created by merging two complementary halves of ellipses with the same vertical axis (1.5 dva) and variable horizontal axes. The width of the resulting asymmetric “egg”-shape was always 1.5 dva, and the degree of asymmetry was adjustable. Finally, a T shape was constructed from a 1.5 dva wide horizontal segment and a 1.5 dva long vertical segment. The horizontal segment was fixed in the middle of the green background ellipse, while the vertical segment could be moved left and right.