In standard visual search, observers search for a target in visual displays containing distractor items (
Wolfe, 2020). In “hybrid search,” observers search the visual display (e.g., the shelves in the supermarket) for a set of possible targets held in memory (e.g., your shopping list). This is known as “hybrid search” because it combines visual and memory search (
Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977;
Wolfe, 2012). It is a typical type of search task encountered in daily life. To investigate hybrid visual and memory search behavior in the laboratory,
Wolfe (2012) asked human observers to memorize 1, 2, 4, 8, 16, or even 100 objects (memory set) prior to search. To confirm that these objects had been firmly stored in their memory, observers completed a simple “old” or “new” memory recognition test. Next, the observers performed repeated trials of visual search through displays consisting of either 1, 2, 4, 8, or 16 photographs of objects (visual set). The observers’ task was to identify if one of the objects in the memory set was present in the search display. The results showed that the search response times (RTs) were a linear function of the number of objects displayed in visual search and a logarithmic function of the number of objects held in memory.
Cunningham and Wolfe (2014) offer a model that interprets the basic mechanism of the interaction between visual and memory search: An object in the visual display is selected. In the
Wolfe (2012) experiment, with a diverse set of target objects, this visual selection will be essentially random (
Wolfe, 2021). That selected item will be compared against the set of target objects held in memory (“memory search”). If it does not match any of them, a new item will be selected. This process will repeat until the selected object matches one of the targets or the search is terminated with a “target absent” response. The time required for each memory search will be a log function of the number of items held in memory. Why is the function logarithmic? One appealing thought is that search through memory is like the child’s game of guessing a number between 1 and
N. A young child will ask, “Is it 1? Is it 2?” and so on; reaching the correct answer in an average of (
N + 1)/2 steps. A wiser child will learn a set partitioning strategy: “Is it bigger than
N/2? If no, is it bigger than
N/4?” and so on, a strategy that requires log2(
N) steps on average. It is difficult to see how that would be implemented in a memory search. A more plausible hypothesis sees the logarithmic function as a by-product of the mechanics of Ratcliff's diffusion model of recognition (
Ratcliff, 1978) or related models. In these models, information about an item accumulates at some rate toward an identification threshold. That threshold should be set to a level that allows for recognition as quickly as possible but not so quickly that a noisy accumulation process will produce a false-positive (false alarm) response. If the target can be any of
N items in a memory set, one can imagine
N diffusion processes accumulating information. The chance of a false positive will go up because each diffuser has some chance of producing a false positive. Accordingly, the recognition threshold should be moved to a higher level if one wishes to avoid an increase in false positives. At a constant average rate of information accumulation, it takes longer to reach a higher threshold.
Leite and Ratcliff (2010) have shown that, if the false-positive rate is held constant, response times will increase logarithmically with the number of diffusers. This seems like a plausible account for the basic hybrid search results.