Participants successfully adapted their history biases to the repeating environment statistics. This is evidenced by the clear shift in the psychometric curves conditioned on the stimulus orientation in the previous trial in the repeating environment (see
Figure 3A). Our multiple logistic regression analysis confirmed that participants increased their bias towards the stimulus orientation of the previous trial in the repeating versus the neutral environment (
Figure 3B, study 1:
t(119) = 9.37,
p < 0.001,
BF10 = 1.09 × 10
13; study 2:
t(70) = 8.33,
p < 0.001,
BF10 = 1.95 × 10
9). Along with this adaptation in the previous stimulus weight, participants also increased their bias to repeat their previous response (
Figure 3B, study 1:
t(119) = 9.61,
p < 0.001, BF
10 = 3.71 × 10
13; study 2:
t(70) = 7.34,
p < 0.001,
BF10 = 3.42 × 10
7). The simultaneous increase of previous stimulus- and choice-weight reflects the participants’ tendency to repeat their choices after correct trials, and a much weaker tendency to switch choices after incorrect trials (
Figure 3B). We therefore focused our subsequent analyses on history biases following correct trials, computed by summing previous stimulus and choice weights. Both previous correct and incorrect weights decay with increasing temporal distance, with
t tests against zero remaining significant for a lag of up to 7 (study 2: 5) trials for the previous correct choice weight and up to 3 (study 2: 4) trials for the previous incorrect choice weight (all
p < 0.05) in the repeating environment (see
Figure 3C). History bias adaptation to the repeating sequence provided an advantage in task performance, driven by an improvement in accuracy in low-contrast trials, defined here as contrasts below 0.06% (see
Figures 3D and
3E). Indeed, the magnitude of the adaptation of the previous correct choice weight correlated with accuracy in low-contrast trials in the repeating environment (study 1:
ρs = 0.49,
p < 0.001,
r = 0.48 [0.32, 0.60],
BF10 = 350428.07; study 2:
ρs = 0.65,
p < 0.001,
r = 0.67 [0.51, 0.78],
BF10 = 9.99 × 10
7). Importantly, it was not possible to predict the rewarded response in 0%-contrast trials in the neutral environment, because these were determined randomly, leading to obligatory chance-level performance of 50% accuracy. Conversely, in the repeating environment, exploiting the transitional structure, which also applied to 0%-contrast trials, allowed participants to increase their accuracy above 50% (
Figure 3E). Overall, our results show that participant successfully adapted their choice history biases to the repeating temporal regularity, thereby improving their perceptual decisions.