Figure 11 illustrates the basic results in CIELAB color space using the green scarf as an example. For this object, under neutral daylight illuminant
, the owner selected chip 5GY5/4 (represented by point A in
Figure 11) as a match to their object. The matches, as well as those under the other illuminants, were made from memory, as the actual object was not present. If this participant were fully color constant, then they would also select chip 5GY5/4 under all four experimental illuminants (represented here, under the purplish illuminant by point B in
Figure 11). If they were less constant they might select an alternative chip such as 2.5GY5/4 (represented by point C in
Figure 2). We computed the corresponding color constancy index (CCI) by projecting vector AC onto AB (indicated by AC’). When C and B are identical, the index equals 1 (expressed as 100%), indicating perfect color constancy. Zero constancy would instead be represented if the observer would pick a chip that has an equal color signal under the purplish illuminant as the memory match under neutral daylight, in this case 5GY7/8 (represented by point D in
Figure 11). Note that this index, usually referred to as Brunswick ratio (BR; see
Foster, 2011) can achieve values larger than 1. We discuss advantages and disadvantages of various indices later on. The main advantage of the BR over other measures is that it is an unbiased estimator in the presence of noise. If the true value of color constancy is 1, then the BR will estimate it to be 1, whereas other indices might underestimate constancy.
Figure 12 shows the results for all objects under the four experimental illuminants, averaged over two sessions. For each object, two lines emerge from the memory color selection under daylight. One line connects the color coordinates of the selected chip under daylight to the color coordinates of the same chip under the new illuminant (represented by a circle). The second line extends to the color coordinate of the chip the observer selected from memory under the new illuminant (represented by a cross).
Figure 12 shows that for most objects these lines stay close together and frequently overlap. Constancy is close to perfect under all conditions, with a mean value of 93.9% (±20%
SD) and a median of 99.2% across all objects and illuminants. There was small variation between illuminants, as shown in
Figure 13, and some variation between objects (and observers), as shown in
Figure 14.
Another way to evaluate constancy is to look at the differences between the perfect match and the selected chip. In the CIE
1976 L*a*b* color space, the Euclidean distance ∆
E to a first approximation represents a perceptual JND, based on measurements by MacAdam (see
Brainard, 2003). From a previous memory study (
Bloj et al., 2016), we know that the reliability of visual long-term memory for our participants and objects is of the order of one Munsell step in hue, one and a half in chroma and half a step in value (see Figures 3 and 4 of
Bloj et al., 2016). This roughly corresponds to the distance between two neighboring Munsell chips, which is roughly equivalent to 5 ∆
E units for the 17 chips selected under neutral daylight in this study. The observed deviations between the selected chip and the chip representing perfect color constancy were small and of the same order as this memory limit (mean ∆
E, 5.5; median ∆
E, 5.14 ± 3.5
SD, ±0.43
SE), as shown in
Figure 15 (see also
Table 2). In 23 out of 132 cases, participants even selected the very same chip under the test illuminant as under natural daylight. The bimodal shape of the histogram arises due to the discreteness of the collection of chips. This means that our observers, with just a few exceptions, were as good as would be expected based on their visual memory.