To examine how the pursuit system responded to the double-drift stimulus, we analyzed the early postsaccadic pursuit movement. The speed of the pursuit from 20 to 80 ms after saccade landing was on average 7.91 dva/s (
SD 1.59), corresponding to a gain of 0.66 (
SD 0.13) from the stimulus speed of 12.0 dva/s. The average speed in the control condition was 8.00 dva/s (
SD 1.42) and 7.81 dva/s (
SD 1.80) in the double-drift condition. A repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) did not reveal significant effects of condition (double-drift vs. control),
F(1, 7) = 0.89,
p = 0.38; direction of motion (inward vs. outward),
F(1, 7) = 1.44,
p = 0.27; or the interaction between condition and direction,
F(1, 7) = 2.50,
p = 0.16, on pursuit speed. This finding suggests that the eye movement behavior did not differ, in the first 100 ms after saccade landings, between the control and double-drift conditions, despite the fact that the moving target was present after the saccade landing only in the control condition (in the double-drift condition, the screen was blank after the saccade). We then restricted the analysis to the double-drift trials. We found that the direction of the pursuit was shifted on average by 25.25° (
SD 3.30) from the physical direction of motion of the target, toward the direction of the internal motion. The size of the shift did not differ across inward versus outward trials,
t(7) = 0.09,
p = 0.93. The deviations of direction measured in pursuit were not different from those measured in the perceptual responses,
t(7) = 0.76,
p = 0.47 (see
Figure 4C). Additionally, there was a tendency for a positive correlation across participants between pursuit and perceptual responses. The correlation was statistically significant with a one-tailed test,
r(7) = 0.69,
p = 0.03. We assessed also whether there were measurable correlations on a trial-by-trial basis. Correlations were quantified using a measure of circular dependence, which has the same properties of the product moment correlation coefficient for linear variables but is appropriate for angular variables (
Jammalamadaka & SenGupta, 1988,
2001). In order to test statistical significance at the group level, individual correlation coefficients were transformed using Fisher’s Z transform (
Fisher, 1915), which transforms the correlation coefficients such that their sampling distribution is approximately normal, and then their 95% confidence intervals were computed. For the correlation between perceptual reports and pursuit, the range was [−0.03, 0.19] with a mean of 0.10 and a 95% CI [0.04, 0.16]. For the correlation between saccade and pursuit, the range was [−0.14, 0.01] with a mean of −0.06 and a 95% CI [−0.10, −0.02]. For the correlation between saccade and perception, the range was [−0.08, 0.14] with a mean of 0.01 and a 95% CI [−0.05, 0.07]. Thus, although the correlation values were generally small (suggesting independent sources of noise affecting each response; see
Supplementary Figure S5), we find some evidence for a correlation between perceptual reports and pursuit, suggesting a similar processing of motion information, and a negative correlation between saccades and pursuit, suggesting cooperative interactions between these two types of eye movements (
Goettker et al., 2018;
Goettker et al., 2019;
Lisi & Cavanagh, 2017b).