We analyzed color in 108 slides of still life paintings (
Table S1 provides the list of works), and 41 color-calibrated photographs of fruit from the McGill Color Calibrated Database (see Methods), examples of which are shown in
Figure 1.
The pixel values of each image, photograph or painting, were analyzed by transforming the chromaticity values into the cone-opponent DKL color space that the retina and lateral geniculate nucleus are thought to use to encode the cone signals (see Methods for details). We used the DKL space because the axis are defined in a physiologically meaningful way and it is well-suited for predicting saturation in natural scenes (
Schiller & Gegenfurtner, 2016).
Figure 2 shows this analysis for a still life painting by Cezanne (
Still Life with Cherries and Peaches, 1887). The top row shows the input image as represented by the two chromatic axes L/M and S and the achromatic axis L/M/S of the DKL color space. The second row shows the distribution of the mean colors in the image projected to the three cardinal planes of DKL. The painting did not have a uniform representation of colors across color space. Instead, there were more pixels with colors in quadrants 1 and 3 of plots with axes of S versus L/M (left), and more dark colors than light colors (middle). This distribution of colors is similar to biases found in natural images (
Webster & Mollon, 1997).
To investigate the distribution of fruit color in paintings and photographs, we computed the joint histograms for the two chromatic DKL axes L/M and S, averaged across all paintings (
Figure 3A) and photographs (
Figure 3B). The joint histograms shown throughout the article bin every pixel of the image as projected to the respective plane. We averaged the individual joint histograms by taking the mean of each bin. In both paintings and photographs, there is a strong bias for pixels in quadrants 1 and 3, as for the example image (
Figure 2).
To investigate the memory color effect in paintings we focused on the relative distribution of pixels in the quadrant that contains fruit-colored pixels (quadrant 1, demarcated with the red arc, containing red, orange, and yellow colors) (
Figures 3A and
3B) versus the three other quadrants (the black arc). The bottom of
Figure 3 shows the relative number of pixels as a function of chromatic contrast. We confirm that fruit colors predominantly lie in the first quadrant by determining the joint distribution of image pixels of ten fruits chosen randomly from the paintings: The joint distributions show a strong bias for quadrant 1, the warm red–orange–yellow colors (
Figure 4).
The number of relatively saturated pixels is greater in quadrant 1 than the other quadrants, for both paintings (
Figure 3D) and photographs (
Figure 3E; the red line extends further to the right of the black line and sits above it). This result shows that, in both paintings and photographs, the fruit colors are relatively more saturated. But the results also show that the relative difference of the number of saturated pixels in quadrant 1 versus the other quadrants is greater for the paintings than the photographs (the red line is further from the black line for the paintings than the photographs) (
Figures 3D and
3E). This result is quantified in
Figure 3F, in which the pink trace depicts for the paintings the relative difference in the number of pixels at each chromatic-contrast level for pixels in quadrant 1 versus the other quadrants, and the blue trace shows the same quantity for the photographs. A comparison of
Figures 3A and
3B also shows a relative increase in purple (quadrant IV) in the paintings compared with the photographs, which may be explained by the increase of violet pigment in the late 19th century (
Reutersvärd, 1950).
Prior work has shown that the parts of scenes that are more likely to be labeled as objects are warm colored rather than cool colored, for both natural objects and artificial objects (
Rosenthal et al., 2018). Accordingly, to test the hypothesis that objects are relatively more saturated than backgrounds in still life paintings, we computed the joint histograms for the L/M vs L/M/S plane. We found that the warm +L/M colors are relatively more saturated than the cool colors (
Figure 5).
We note that the results are also consistent with the conclusion that cool colors, typically associated with backgrounds, are relatively less saturated in the paintings. Blue is a commonly preferred color (
Eysenck, 1941;
McManus, Jones, & Cottrell, 1981;
Palmer & Schloss, 2010), so if the effect we found for “warm” colors simply reflects color preference, it should also show up for the “blue” pixels. To test this hypothesis, we computed the joint histograms for the S plane versus the L/M/S plane and evaluated the relative chromatic contrast of pixels in the “blue” quadrant (
Figure 6). As shown in
Figure 6, the relative chromatic contrast of blue pixels versus other colored pixels is no greater for paintings than photographs. We conclude that the effect we found is specific for colors in the first quadrant and cannot be explained by color preference.
Finally, we analyzed the chromatic and achromatic differences across the images, inspired by prior work that hypothesizes that visual discomfort is related to the local differences in luminance and chromaticity across an image (
Penacchio et al., 2021). Penacchio et al. found support for this hypothesis in paintings and photographs of natural scenes, with a notable exception of fruit. The data set we analyzed is enriched for representations of fruit, providing an opportunity to further explore this potential exception and the generalizability of the hypothesis.
Figure 7 shows for the still life paintings and the photographs the differences in CIE chromaticity (L, u′, and v′; as done by Penacchio et al.). The mean achromatic vs chromatic differences for photographs and paintings are indicated by the large crosses. The achromatic and chromatic differences are highly correlated for photographs,
r = 0.68;
p < 1e-6, but not for paintings,
r = 0.007;
p = 0.48, suggesting that a cognitive act of the artists is engaged to decouple color contrast from luminance contrast. The results also suggest that paintings made more recently (more contemporary vs. baroque) have higher achromatic contrast (the blue dots to the right of the convex hull for the photographs). In addition, it also seems that paintings of a single painter sometimes cluster (like for Jan Davidsz. De Heem, the yellow dots around [0.01, 0.015]), suggesting that this parameter may reflect an artistic fingerprint. Interestingly, the outlier of the Baroque paintings is Caravaggio's
Basket of Fruit, which is the only painting that Caravaggio painted with a light background.