As observers are given unlimited time to make these visual judgments, how often they switch between observing one object or another may reveal task-specific biases.
Figure 5C shows the average switch rate (switches per second) made for the different tasks. A switch was considered to take place when a saccade began on one object and ended on the other object. Switch rates did not show a main effect of task,
F(2, 28) = 0.96,
p = 0.40, with no main effect of boosted stimuli,
F(1, 14) = 1.84,
p = 0.12, and no interaction,
F(2, 28) = 1.03,
p = 0.37. Switch rates are remarkably similar between different tasks and conditions, but are observers spending the same amount of time on different tasks? The average manual response time (RT) for the different tasks and conditions tested is shown in
Figure 5D. RT shows a main effect of task,
F(2, 28) = 19.05,
\({\it p} \lt 0.001, \eta _{p}^{2}=\) 0.30, without a main effect of boosted stimuli,
F(1, 14) = 3.62,
\({\it p} = 0.08, \eta _{p}^{2}=\) 0.21, and a significant interaction,
F(2, 28) = 5.85,
\({\it p} = 0.008, \eta _{p}^{2}=\) 0.30. Brightness judgments are associated with shorter RTs than width and height judgments (all
p < 0.001). Additionally, height judgments for boosted trials show longer RTs than for unboosted trials (
p = 0.01). In contrast with
Figure 5D, the rate of switches per second is comparable across tasks. This means that height judgments with boosted stimuli will have more switches per trial, but this arises from a longer trial duration. Additionally, brightness judgments show the fastest RTs but the longest fixation durations, suggesting that observers make fewer but longer fixations for brightness judgments.