We separated the data based on whether the participant made a microsaccade during the critical period for performance (between ready signal onset and stimulus offset; duration = 590 ms) or not. Based on this criterion, the number of trials with a MS (12,789 in heterogeneous and 12,051 in homogeneous conditions) and without (12,583 in heterogeneous and 12,402 in homogeneous conditions) was similar in both conditions across all participants. A three-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) (2 [Experiment 1 vs. 2] × 2 [MS presence vs. absence] × 4 [locations]) revealed a significant three-way interaction (
F(3, 81) = 53.06,
p < 0.001,
η2G = 0.373) (
Figure 4A). For Experiment 1, a two-way ANOVA yielded main effects of location (
F(3, 81) = 81.69,
p < 0.001,
η2G = 0.666), showing that both asymmetries were present (RHM, LHM > UVM, LVM,
p < 0.001; LVM > UVM,
p < 0.001), and microsaccade presence (
F(1, 27) = 17.70,
p < 0.001,
η2G = 0.033) but no interaction between them. Performance was better in the absence (1.56 ± 0.05) than presence (1.36 ± 0.07) of microsaccades (
Figure 5B, left panel). A paired-samples
t-test conducted to compare the
d′ means on Experiment 1 trials with and without microsaccades revealed that the difference was significant (
t(27) = 2.57,
p = 0.01,
d = 0.52). For Experiment 2, a two-way ANOVA yielded only a main effect of microsaccade presence (
F(1, 27) = 20.09,
p < 0.001,
η2G = 0.066). Again, performance was better in the absence (1.60 ± 0.05) than presence (1.27 ± 0.07) of microsaccades (
Figure 4B, right panel) during the critical period. A paired-samples
t-test comparing performance on Experiment 2 trials with and without microsaccades revealed that the difference between the groups was significant (
t(27) = 5.27,
p < 0.001,
d = 1). Regarding RT (
Figure 4C), neither the interactions nor the main effects were significant (all
p > 0.05). Thus, there were no speed–accuracy trade-offs.