Open Access
Article  |   August 2024
Perception of #TheDress in childhood is influenced by age and green-leaf preference
Author Affiliations
  • Guillermo Salcedo-Villanueva
    Retina Department, Asociación Para Evitar la Ceguera en México, IAP, Mexico City, Mexico
    guillermo.salcedo@apec.com.mx
  • Catalina Becerra-Revollo
    Ocular Ultrasound Department, Asociación Para Evitar la Ceguera en México, IAP, Mexico City, Mexico
    catabec@gmail.com
  • Luis Antonio Rhoads-Avila
    Retina Department, Asociación Para Evitar la Ceguera en México, IAP, Mexico City, Mexico
    rhoadsluis@gmail.com
  • Julian García-Sánchez
    Retina Department, Asociación Para Evitar la Ceguera en México, IAP, Mexico City, Mexico
    juliangarcia011@gmail.com
  • Flor Angélica Jácome-Gutierrez
    Retina Department, Asociación Para Evitar la Ceguera en México, IAP, Mexico City, Mexico
    jacomegutierrezflorangelica@gmail.com
  • Linda Cernichiaro-Espinosa
    Retina Department, Asociación Para Evitar la Ceguera en México, IAP, Mexico City, Mexico
    linda.cernichiaro@gmail.com
  • Andrée Henaine-Berra
    Hospital San Angel Inn Universidad, Mexico City, Mexico
    andreehenaine@gmail.com
  • Axel Orozco-Hernandez
    Clínica de Retina, Guadalajara, Mexico
    axeloh@yahoo.com
  • Humberto Ruiz-García
    Clínica Santa Lucía, Guadalajara, Mexico
    hruizgarcia@gmail.com
  • Eduardo Torres-Porras
    Provissia, Puebla, Mexico
    retinapuebla@yahoo.com.mx
Journal of Vision August 2024, Vol.24, 11. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.8.11
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Guillermo Salcedo-Villanueva, Catalina Becerra-Revollo, Luis Antonio Rhoads-Avila, Julian García-Sánchez, Flor Angélica Jácome-Gutierrez, Linda Cernichiaro-Espinosa, Andrée Henaine-Berra, Axel Orozco-Hernandez, Humberto Ruiz-García, Eduardo Torres-Porras; Perception of #TheDress in childhood is influenced by age and green-leaf preference. Journal of Vision 2024;24(8):11. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.8.11.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

The perception of the ambiguous image of #TheDress may be influenced by optical factors, such as macular pigments. Their accumulation during childhood could increase with age and the ingestion of carotenoid-containing foods. The purpose of this study was to investigate whether the visual perception of the dress in children would differ based on age and carotenoid preference. This was a cross-sectional, observational, and comparative study. A poll was administered to children aged 2 to 10 years. Parents were instructed to inquire about the color of #TheDress from their children. A carotenoid preference survey was also completed. A total of 413 poll responses were analyzed. Responses were categorized based on the perceived color of the dress: blue/black (BB) (n = 204) and white/gold (WG) (n = 209). The mean and median age of the WG group was higher than the BB group (mean 6.1, median 6.0 years, standard deviation [SD] 2.2; mean 5.5, median 5.0 years, SD 2.3; p = 0.007). Spearman correlation between age and group was 0.133 (p = 0.007). Green-leaf preference (GLP) showed a statistically significant difference between groups (Mann–Whitney U: p = 0.038). Spearman correlation between GLP and group was 0.102 (p = 0.037). Logistic regression for the perception of the dress as WG indicated that age and GLP were significant predictors (age: B weight 0.109, p = 0.012, odds ratio: 1.115; GLP: B weight 0.317, p = 0.033, odds ratio: 1.373). Older children and those with a higher GLP were more likely to perceive #TheDress as WG. These results suggest a potential relationship with the gradual accumulation of macular pigments throughout a child's lifetime.

Introduction
In 2015, an image was published on the Internet under the name #TheDress (Figure 1), available at https://web.archive.org/web/20150227014959/http://swiked.tumblr.com/post/112073818575/guys-please-help-me-is-this-dress-white-and. This image went viral, sparking controversy over how people perceived its color. Some saw it as blue and black (BB), while others reported it as white and gold (WG). The diverse opinions on the color perception of the dress piqued scientific curiosity, leading to efforts to explain the reasons behind such contrasting visual experiences of a single image among different individuals. 
Figure 1.
 
Image of #TheDress. Most people perceive it either as blue/black or white/gold (https://web.archive.org/web/20150227014959/http://swiked.tumblr.com/post/112073818575/guys-please-help-me-is-this-dress-white-and). Copyright Cecilia Bleasdale.
Figure 1.
 
Image of #TheDress. Most people perceive it either as blue/black or white/gold (https://web.archive.org/web/20150227014959/http://swiked.tumblr.com/post/112073818575/guys-please-help-me-is-this-dress-white-and). Copyright Cecilia Bleasdale.
Several published studies have proposed hypotheses to explain the ambiguous perception of the dress. One explanation involves the subjectivity of an individual's perception, which can be attributed to color constancy. Color constancy is the perceptual phenomenon that keeps object colors stable under variable lighting conditions (Hurlbert, 2007). For instance, if a person views a red apple under bluish-hued light, the light the apple reflects would not be the same as from an apple under white light, but the person will still perceive the apple as red. 
The challenge in understanding color constancy comes from the assumption that observers might perceive colors based on different internal criteria. As stated by Foster (2011), “[Observers] … may draw on different invariant properties of images; and their ability to extract those invariants may depend on other image properties that covary with illuminant changes.” The spectral irradiance from the source of illumination and the reflectance of a surface are difficult to estimate from the spectral radiance that is reaching the eye. 
The dress has been scrutinized through the lens of color constancy, with studies suggesting that subjects may assume the object (the dress) is under either direct lighting or in shade (Brainard & Hurlbert, 2015; Winkler, Spillmann, Werner, & Webster, 2015). Various studies have explored color constancy by filtering and modifying the brightness and spatial frequency of the image. The visual system appears to extract both low and high spatial frequencies from the environment, contributing to the observed variability in individual perceptions (Dixon & Shapiro, 2017). 
In an intriguing experiment, Dixon and Shapiro (2017) demonstrated that a filter-based approach, specifically by filtering low spatial frequencies (removing the illuminant), resulted in the perception of the dress as WG. This finding contradicted the anticipated outcome, as the removal of the illuminant was expected to lead to the perception of the object as BB. This suggests a potential effect caused by the filter or the attenuation of the illuminant (Dixon & Shapiro, 2017). 
As explained by Gonzalez Martín-Moro et al., this theory does not fully elucidate whether individuals deduce the lighting conditions of a scene first or base their color deductions on the perceived illumination (Chetverikov & Ivanchei, 2016; Gonzalez Martín-Moro et al., 2018). It remains challenging to determine why some individuals assume the lighting of a scene differently from others (Gonzalez-Martín-Moro et al., 2021). 
The neural mechanisms that support color constancy are thought to include chromatic adaptation in the retina (Creutzfeldt, Crook, Kastner, Li, & Pei, 1991; Kamermans, Kraaij, & Spekreijse, 1998; Neumeyer, Dörr, Fritsch, & Kardelky, 2002), spatial comparisons between chromatic signals in the lateral geniculate nucleus and V1 (Moutoussis & Zeki, 2000; Nascimento & Foster, 2001), and surround interactions as observed in V1 (Wachtler, Sejnowski, & Albright, 2003) and V4 areas of the visual cortex (Schein & Desimone, 1990; Walsh, Carden, Butler, & Kulikowski, 1993). 
Biological mechanisms at higher and lower levels have also been implicated in the perception of #TheDress: Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies analyzing neuronal pathways revealed higher activation in the frontal and parietal lobes in subjects perceiving the dress as WG (Schlaffke et al., 2015). Ocular variables, such as pupil size, indicated that WG perceivers had a smaller average pupil size (Vemuri, Bisla, Mulpuru, & Varadharajan, 2016; Vemuri, Srivastava, Agrawal, & Anand, 2018). Age, hyperopia, and lens opacity were associated with BB perception (González-Martín-Moro et al., 2021). 
Another biological factor that could contribute to the visual perception of #TheDress is the “early-stage” processing in the retina due to a higher concentration of macular pigments (MPs), as reported by Rabin, Houser, Talbert, and Patel (2016). Their study found that subjects with high MPs were more likely to perceive the dress as WG, while those with lower pigment concentrations observed it as BB. The authors concluded that macular pigment optical density (MPOD) and cone input may play a role in defining the color of an ambiguous visual perception, such as the dress. 
MPs—namely, lutein, zeaxanthin, and meso-zeaxanthin—are xanthophyll carotenoids acquired through the diet (Bone, Landrum, Hime, Cains, & Zamor, 1993). Functioning as filters for visible light with high frequency/low wavelengths, MPs also act as scavengers for reactive oxygen species (ROS) (Krinsky, Landrum, & Bone, 2003). Their strategic location in the fovea is crucial due to a high propensity to generate ROS (Cai, Nelson, Wu, Sternberg, & Jones, 2000; Sujak et al., 1999). Numerous studies have explored carotenoid sources in the diet (Perry, Rasmussen, & Johnson, 2009; Rodriguez-Amaya, 2003), the optical properties conferred by macular pigments enhancing contrast sensitivity through prereceptoral filtration of blue wavelengths (Hammond, Wooten, Engles, & Wong, 2012), and their impact on neuronal processing (Renzi, Bovier, & Hammond, 2013). High levels of carotenoids (lutein and zeaxanthin) have been associated with a significantly lower risk of developing advanced age-related macular degeneration (AMD) (Age-Related Eye Disease Study 2 Research Group, 2013; Seddon et al., 1994). 
As MP concentration relies on ingestion, two factors may contribute to their increase: the intake of foods rich in carotenoids, such as green-leafy vegetables, fruits, corn, egg yolks, and salmon, and a gradual accumulation with age, provided intake remains sufficient and constant. 
If the color perception of #TheDress is partially influenced by MP levels, as proposed by Rabin et al. (2016), two hypotheses can be considered. First, a younger individual with inadequate carotenoid consumption may perceive the image as BB, while an older individual, having had more time for MP accumulation, might observe it as WG. Second, individuals with a higher intake of carotenoid-containing foods may perceive it as WG, while those with inadequate carotenoid consumption may report it as BB. 
To address these hypotheses, we chose to analyze very young individuals during the crucial years of MP accumulation. Utilizing the image of the dress was particularly useful, as asking a child about the color of an object is one of the simplest questions a toddler can answer. This allowed us to inquire about how children aged 2 to 10 perceived the dress, studying visual perception in relation to external factors like carotenoids and the consequent accumulation of MPs in the first decade of life. By asking parents about their child's carotenoid preference, representing subjective fondness and likelihood to consume carotenoid-containing food, we aimed to obtain an overall idea of carotenoid intake. 
Therefore, the purpose of this research was to investigate whether the visual perception of the dress in children differs between younger and older age groups and between those with low and high carotenoid preferences. 
Method
Study design and ethical considerations
This cross-sectional, observational, and comparative study aimed to analyze how age and carotenoid preference influence the perception of the dress in children. Institutional review board (IRB) approval was obtained before data collection. Parents provided written informed consent for their children; the children also were given informed assent forms to participate in the study. The form included the publication of results. The study adhered to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and its revisions. 
Data collection
Children aged 2 to 10 years were studied using an IRB-approved Internet poll. The poll was distributed to parents, who were instructed to ask their children the specific question, “What is the color of this dress?” without providing color options to avoid influencing the answer. Parents recorded the exact response given by their child in the poll. If multiple children in a family met the age criteria, parents were instructed to ask each child separately to prevent bias from siblings. Parents were also asked if their children had seen the image before, and only subjects who had not seen the image (naive subjects) were included. 
In a separate section, parents were queried about their child's carotenoid preference. The “Carotenoid Preference Survey” assessed preferences for five foods containing carotenoids: (1) green-leafy vegetables, (2) fruits, (3) corn, (4) egg (specifically egg yolk), and (5) salmon or rainbow trout. Parents rated consumption as (A) “Does not eat,” (B) “Eats in small amounts,” or (C) “Eats in good amounts,” with corresponding points assigned (A = 1, B = 2, C = 3). Two main outcomes were derived from the Carotenoid Preference Survey: total carotenoid preference (sum of all sources) and green-leaf preference (points obtained from green-leafy vegetables), considering the latter as the primary outcome for dietary analysis due to its association with macular pigments (Böhm et al., 2021). 
Statistical analysis
The sample size for the poll was calculated using the formula for surveys studying qualitative variables (Charan & Biswas, 2013):  
\begin{eqnarray*} \rm {Sample \, size} = \frac {\it Z \rm 1- \it a/ \rm 2^2 \it p(\rm 1- \it p)}{\it d2} \end{eqnarray*}
 
The calculated sample size targeted 385 participants. The poll was administered using Google Forms. Results were categorized into two groups: Group 1 BB and Group 2 WG. Only answers that that fitted these two possibilities of colors (BB and WG) were included in the final analysis. Other color responses were eliminated. 
For nonnormally distributed variables, the Mann–Whitney U test was employed. Correlations were analyzed using Pearson's correlation and Spearman's rho. Logistic regression models were designed to obtain odds ratios for significant variables. Data were recorded in spreadsheets, and statistical analysis was performed using SPSS (Ver. 22; IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). 
Results
Following our sample size calculation of 385, we successfully obtained responses from 447 parents with children. From this pool, we analyzed 413 poll responses, consisting of 219 (53%) females and 194 (46%) males. Responses were excluded if they were duplicated or incomplete or if the child reported a color other than BB or WG, such as red, green, or pink. A total of 10 children (2.2% of the complete survey sample) reported a different color or combination of colors: red/green (1), green (3), brown (1), red (2), pink/brown (1), pink/green (1), and brilliant (1). 
Age distribution in the complete sample
The mean age in the complete sample was 5.8 years, with a standard deviation (SD) of 2.3, a median of 5 years, and a 95% confidence interval (CI) of 5.5–6.0. The distribution across different ages was as follows: 2 years (24 children), 3 years (50 children), 4 years (62 children), 5 years (75 children), 6 years (51 children), 7 years (39 children), 8 years (44 children), 9 years (28 children), and 10 years (40 children). 
Comparison of age between color perception groups
Responses were categorized into two groups based on the perceived color of the dress: Group 1 BB (n = 204) and Group 2 WG (n = 209). Group BB had a mean age of 5.5 years (SD: 2.3), a median of 5 years, and a 95% CI of 5.1–5.8. Group WG had a mean age of 6.1 years (SD: 2.2), a median of 6 years, and a 95% CI of 5.8–6.4 (refer to Figure 2). 
Figure 2.
 
Line graph of age distribution between groups of perception. Notice the higher perception of blue/black at 2 years and how white/gold increases through time. A statistically significant difference was found between groups (p = 0.007). Two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test: p = 0.049.
Figure 2.
 
Line graph of age distribution between groups of perception. Notice the higher perception of blue/black at 2 years and how white/gold increases through time. A statistically significant difference was found between groups (p = 0.007). Two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test: p = 0.049.
Both age samples were assessed for normality using the Shapiro–Wilk test, indicating a nonnormal distribution for both groups. To compare the mean age between groups, the Mann–Whitney U test was employed, resulting in mean ranks for age of 191.11 for Group BB and 222.51 for Group WG. The Mann–Whitney U statistic was 18,076.500 (p = 0.007), indicating a statistically significant older age in Group WG compared to Group BB. Groups were ranked based on our hypothesis, with Group BB having a value of 1 and Group WG having a value of 2. Spearman's correlation between age and group revealed a low, positive correlation for WG (r = 0.133, p = 0.007). 
Carotenoid preference analysis
For each food group in the Carotenoid Preference Survey, points ranged from a minimum of 1 to a maximum of 3. The total carotenoid preference (TCP), representing the sum of points across all food groups, ranged from 5 to 15 points. 
TCP by groups
Results for TCP were divided into groups based on the perceived color of the dress. Group BB had a mean TCP of 12.52 (SD: 1.7), a median of 13, and a 95% CI of 12.28–12.77. Group WG had a mean TCP of 12.82 (SD: 1.5), a median of 13, and a 95% CI of 12.6–13.03 (refer to Figure 3). A Mann–Whitney U test was used to compare mean TCP between groups, resulting in mean ranks of 199.26 for Group BB and 214.55 for Group WG. The Mann–Whitney U statistic was 19,739.500 (p = 0.185). Consequently, no significant difference was observed in TCP between the two groups. 
Figure 3.
 
Line graph of total carotenoid preference between groups of perception. No statistically significant difference was found when comparing the two groups (p = 0.185).
Figure 3.
 
Line graph of total carotenoid preference between groups of perception. No statistically significant difference was found when comparing the two groups (p = 0.185).
Green-leaf preference analysis
Considering the variation in carotenoid concentration across food groups, particularly with the highest concentration in green-leafy vegetables (Perry et al., 2009), we explored differences in green-leaf preference (GLP) between groups. 
Overall GLP distribution
From the 413 polls, 44 children (10.7%) reported not consuming green-leafy vegetables, 132 (32%) reported consuming them in small amounts, and 237 (57%) reported consuming them in good amounts. 
GLP by perceived dress color groups
When polls were categorized by dress color perception groups, Group BB reported 28 cases (13.7%) of not consuming green-leafy vegetables, 68 cases (33.3%) reported consuming them in small amounts, and 108 cases (52.9%) reported consuming them in good amounts. Group WG reported 16 cases (7.7%) of not consuming green-leafy vegetables, 64 cases (30.6%) reported consuming them in small amounts, and 129 cases (61.7%) reported consuming them in good amounts (refer to Figure 4). 
Figure 4.
 
Bar graph depicting the distribution of green-leaf preference (GLP). Notice how children who prefer to eat green leaves perceive the colors more often as white/gold (Mann–Whitney U = 19,094.0; p = 0.038), indicating a statistically significant higher GLP in the white/gold group.
Figure 4.
 
Bar graph depicting the distribution of green-leaf preference (GLP). Notice how children who prefer to eat green leaves perceive the colors more often as white/gold (Mann–Whitney U = 19,094.0; p = 0.038), indicating a statistically significant higher GLP in the white/gold group.
Statistical tests for GLP
A Mann–Whitney U test was used to compare ranks of GLP between groups. Group BB had a mean rank of 196.10, and Group WG had a mean rank of 217.64. The Mann–Whitney U statistic was 19,094.0 (p = 0.038), indicating a statistically significant higher GLP in Group WG. Spearman's rho between GLP and group showed a low positive correlation (r = 0.102, p = 0.037). Additionally, a chi-square test, linear-by-linear association, yielded a statistic of 4.916 (1) with a p-value of 0.027, further supporting the significance of the association. 
Logistic regression analysis
A logistic regression model was employed to determine the odds ratios for age and GLP in predicting the perception of the dress as WG. The model summary resulted in a Nagelkerke R2 of 0.036. The constant was –1.073, Wald 9.592, p = 0.002, odds ratio (Exp B) = 0.342. 
Age predicting dress perception as WG
Age demonstrated a B weight of 0.109, Wald 6.325, yielding a statistically significant result (p = 0.012). The odds ratio for age was 1.115, with a 95% CI of 1.024–1.213. 
GLP predicting dress perception as WG
GLP exhibited a B weight of 0.317, Wald 4.562, resulting in a statistically significant finding (p = 0.033). The odds ratio for GLP was 1.373, with a 95% CI of 1.026–1.836. 
With this model, the predicted probability of getting a WG response based on age and GLP was 60.3% (BB Group predicted probability was 53.4%), with an overall probability of 56.9% (against a null hypothesis overall prediction of 50.6%). Pearson correlation for group and age resulted in 0.127 (p = 0.010); Pearson correlation for group and GLP resulted in 0.109 (p = 0.026). 
Discussion
In this study, we observed that a child's age and preference for ingesting carotenoid-containing foods, particularly green leaves, can influence the perception of an ambiguous image like the dress. As hypothesized, these results suggest a potential relationship with the gradual accumulation of MPs throughout a child's lifetime. 
Lutein, zeaxanthin, and meso-zeaxanthin, the constituents of MPs, accumulate at high concentrations in the fovea, constituting the majority of retinal carotenoids (Landrum & Bone, 2001). Various tissues, including the retina, rely on the dietary intake of these carotenoids for their accumulation. Their concentrations have been identified in maternal blood and umbilical cord blood samples, with lutein and zeaxanthin prevailing in these tissues despite not being the most abundant carotenoids in serum or diet (Thoene et al., 2019). Additionally, further research has highlighted these two carotenoids as the primary ones in human milk (Canfield et al., 2003; Lipkie, Morrow, Jouni, McMahon, & Ferruzzi, 2015). 
As individuals age, the levels of MPs tend to increase. Notably, in newborn and fetal human eyes, a low ratio of meso-zeaxanthin to lutein/zeaxanthin has been observed, suggesting potential insufficiency and/or inefficiency in isomerization (Krinsky et al., 2003). The earliest measurements of MPs in newborns were reported by Sasano et al., who recorded an MPOD of 0.05 at 33 weeks of postmenstrual age (PMA) and a mean first measurable MPOD at 0.076. Their study demonstrated a significant correlation between MPOD levels and PMA, indicating that MPOD increases with age in premature infants (R² = 0.91, p = 0.0001). By 40 weeks of PMA, the mean MPOD reached 0.1033 (Sasano et al., 2018). Bernstein et al. reported a steady rise in MPOD over the first 7 years of life, ranging from 0.05 to nearly 0.4, with linear regression analysis predicting an MPOD of 0.0835 at birth. The MPOD levels eventually reached those found in adult populations (Bernstein et al., 2013). These observations support our hypothesis that older children are more likely to perceive #TheDress as WG, potentially due to a higher accumulation of MPs. This aligns with findings from Lafer-Sousa et al., who reported that older individuals and women were more likely to perceive the image as WG (Lafer-Sousa, Hermann, & Conway, 2015). It is noteworthy that MPs continue to increase during adulthood, reaching maximal levels between 30 years (Hong, Jung, Lee, & Chang, 2020) and 55 years (van der Veen et al., 2009), followed by a gradual decline in older ages. Van der Veen et al. reported higher MPODs in women compared to men across all age groups, except for those aged 80 and above. This observation parallels the tendency of women to perceive more WG, aligning with the decline in WG perceptions through age reported by Gonzalez Martin-Miron and Wallisch, as further discussed in the following sections. 
Research indicates that MP carotenoids contribute to improved visual and cognitive performance across various age groups. A meta-analysis of 9 clinical trials demonstrated that carotenoid supplementation enhances cognitive performance in adults aged 45 to 78 years (Davinelli, Ali, Solfrizzi, Scapagnini, & Corbi, 2021). Studies on young adults showed improved visual task performance with glare disability and photostress recovery after 6 months of carotenoid supplementation (Stringham & Hammond, 2008). Furthermore, a randomized, double-masked, placebo-controlled trial revealed that lutein and zeaxanthin supplementation in healthy young adults led to enhancements in visual memory, complex attention, and reasoning ability. Notably, visual memory exhibited the most significant improvement, suggesting the influence of these carotenoids on functions predominantly mediated by the occipital cortex, a region accumulating lutein and zeaxanthin (Renzi-Hammond et al., 2017). In children, MPOD positively correlated with cognitive control performance (Walk et al., 2017). In a prospective cohort study, higher maternal intake of lutein and zeaxanthin during the first and second trimesters of pregnancy was associated with better verbal intelligence, behavioral regulation ability in mid-childhood, and improved social-emotional development (Mahmassani et al., 2021). Additionally, higher maternal zeaxanthin concentrations were linked to a lower risk of poor visual acuity in children (Lai et al., 2020). 
The perception of #TheDress appears to be influenced by early-stage optical, retinal, and neural factors, as proposed by Rabin et al. (2016). Adult subjects with higher MP levels tended to perceive the image as white and gold, demonstrating differences in processing times, possibly attributed to varying MP concentrations. Visual evoked potentials further indicated distinct neural responses for those perceiving the image as white and gold, suggesting a correlation with MP levels (Rabin et al., 2016). fMRI studies on #TheDress revealed increased brain activation in regions such as the middle frontal gyrus, inferior and superior parietal lobule, middle temporal gyrus, and inferior frontal gyrus among individuals perceiving the dress as WG (Schlaffke et al., 2015). The occipital and frontal cortex, which contain lutein and zeaxanthin, exhibited enhanced activation in these studies, emphasizing the association between carotenoids and specific brain regions crucial for visual processing and higher cognitive function (Vishwanathan, Neuringer, Snodderly, Schalch, & Johnson, 2013). This correlation suggests that lutein and zeaxanthin may play a role in promoting brain health and cognition by supporting neural structures and enhancing neuronal efficiency in regions involved in visual perception and decision-making (Mewborn et al., 2018). 
Other ocular variables, such as hyperopia, cataract density, and age, have been previously associated with the perception of #TheDress (González-Martín-Moro et al., 2021). However, our observations differ from those reported by Gonzalez Martin-Moro et al., where all three variables were associated with perceiving the image as BB. In their study involving a sample of 1,092 subjects aged 2 to 99 years, the authors noted that in the first decade of life, perceived colors were approximately 60% WG and 40% BB (we reported 50.6% WG and 49.3% BB). The authors were unable to explain the association of age with BB perception and concluded that ocular examination did not contribute to understanding the visual phenomenon. Here, we propose the gradual accumulation of MPs during childhood as a potential factor associated with perception. This hypothesis aligns with the concept of stability in perception as children reach MP optical density levels comparable to adults, as reported by Bernstein et al. (2013). The life cycle analysis of #TheDress perception supports this idea, demonstrating variations through age. In a cohort of 7,868 subjects, those perceiving the dress as WG reached a proportion of around 60% after adolescence, remaining stable for approximately 20 years. Subsequently, the proportion dropped to 30% between the ages of 65 and 75 (Wallisch, 2017), a decline also observed after the sixth decade of life in Gonzalez-Martin-Moro's report of 1,025 subjects (Gonzalez-Martin-Moro et al., 2021). Notably, our study suggests an apparent increase in WG perceptions as age advances from 2 to 10 years, prompting interest in understanding the trend beyond the age of 10. 
In examining the relationship between GLP and the perception of #TheDress as WG, it is crucial to note that green-leafy vegetables serve as the primary source of lutein and zeaxanthin. These carotenoids, crucial for MPs, are found in abundance in green leaves, making them significant contributors to visual processes (Böhm et al., 2021; Estévez-Santiago, Beltrán-de-Miguel, & Olmedilla-Alonso, 2016; Franke, Fröhlich, Werner, Böhm, & Schöne, 2010; Sommerburg, Keunen, Bird, & van Kuijk, 1998). While oils, eggs, and fish contribute to carotenoid intake, they appear to be less significant in comparison (Nolan et al., 2014). In our study, GLP was chosen as the primary outcome for analysis, given the crucial role of green-leafy vegetables as principal contributors to MPs. Even though TCP did not yield a statistically significant difference, GLP showed a significant correlation with perceiving #TheDress as WG. This aligns with previous research indicating positive correlations between dietary lutein and zeaxanthin, serum levels, and MPOD, particularly driven by vegetable and fruit consumption (Burke, Curran-Celentano, & Wenzel, 2005). Burke et al. reported that individuals with lower fruit and vegetable consumption had lower MPOD, while those with higher consumption exhibited higher MPOD levels (r = 0.35, p = 0.001). Additionally, their study highlighted a decrease in MPOD with age, reinforcing the link between age and macular health (Burke et al., 2005). Observing GLP provides valuable insights into macular health and, in this context, correlates with the perception of #TheDress, offering a glimpse into the potential influence of dietary factors on visual experiences. 
Our study has several limitations. First, it employs a cross-sectional analysis, and second, the sample size is relatively small when compared to larger Internet surveys (Lafer-Sousa et al., 2015; Wallisch, 2017). These issues may lead to a cohort effect, potentially affecting our sample of children, particularly in the age analysis. Third, while we believe this study provides crucial insights into visual perception and potential mechanisms related to MPs, it may be considered a “low-level” effect influencing the colors of the dress (Wallisch, 2017). It has been suggested that almost 70% of factors influencing the perception of the dress could be environmental rather than genetic (Mahroo et al., 2017). Some of the “low-level” factors influencing the sensory end of perception, as proposed by Wallisch, could be the MPOD, as suggested by Rabin et al. (2016). Our study aligns with these conclusions. However, results from these low-level factors neither explain nor demonstrate a high correlation with the perception. In contrast, the perceived direction of the light source illuminating the dress has been linked to the precept (Chetverikov & Ivanchei, 2016; Wallisch, 2017), where an assumed shadow cast on the dress would make it appear “yellower” through color constancy mechanisms (Wallisch, 2017; Witzel, Racey, & O'Regan, 2017). Although we did not inquire about the children's assumptions regarding the light direction on the dress in our study, analyzing this in a cohort of children could be challenging, especially with 2-year-olds. 
Another factor found by Wallisch (2017) to be related “in a dose-dependent fashion” is the circadian type of each subject, classified as either “owls” or “larks.” This observation also relates to the perception of the illumination on the dress. Exploring how the circadian rhythm of children changes and transitions from “larks” toward “owls” (Randler, Faßl, & Kalb, 2017) and its relation to their perception could shed light on the observed shift toward WG in our sample. These findings may also be related to the effects achieved by action potentials emerging from intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs). Through their absorption of blue wavelengths and using melanopsin, ipRGCs are known to influence conscious visual perception, circadian rhythm, and photo-entrainment (Dacey et al., 2005; Do, 2019; Rabin et al., 2023). 
Finally, could some of the mechanisms proposed in our study affect perception in older ages? As observed, a decrease in WG perceptions happens around 60 years of age (Wallisch, 2017). Older age and the grade of nuclear opacity were associated with BB perception. Additionally, BB perceptions were correlated with unhealthy ocular conditions, such as glaucoma, AMD, and other retinal diseases (Person's correlation coefficient 0.64) (Gonzalez-Martin-Moro et al., 2021). Could a decrease in MPs at an older age partially influence BB perceptions? This question warrants investigation through direct MP measurements, as observed in cohorts, such as patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer's disease, displaying lower levels of MPs, poorer visual function, and an increased risk of AMD (Nolan, Loskutova et al., 2014). 
Conclusions
The dress continues to be an intriguing and captivating illusion, displaying a general bimodal distribution of observed colors (Lafer-Sousa & Conway, 2017) and maintaining stability in perception even from a “one-shot experience” (Drissi-Daoudi, Doerig, Parkosadze, Kunchulia, & Herzog, 2020). This stability may be influenced by higher-level representations of colors (Retter, Gwinn, O'Neil, Jiang, & Webster, 2020). Our study indicates that the bimodal perception of #TheDress is commonly observed in children aged 2 to 10. Furthermore, age and GLP likely influence the perception of its colors, potentially linked to an increased accumulation of MPs. 
Acknowledgments
Commercial relationships: Guillermo Salcedo-Villanueva is a consultant for IOSA Health and Janssen. Axel Orozco-Hernandez, Humberto Ruiz-García, and Eduardo Torres-Porras are speakers for Carl-Zeiss. The rest of the authors have no commercial relationships. 
Corresponding author: Guillermo Salcedo-Villanueva. 
Email: guillermo.salcedo@apec.com.mx. 
Address: Retina Department, Asociación Para Evitar la Ceguera en México, IAP, Mexico City, Mexico. 
References
Age-Related Eye Disease Study 2 Research Group. (2013). Lutein + zeaxanthin and omega-3 fatty acids for age-related macular degeneration: The Age-Related Eye Disease Study 2 (AREDS2) randomized clinical trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 309(19), 2005–2015.
Bernstein, P. S., Sharifzadeh, M., Liu, A., Ermakov, I., Nelson, K., Sheng, X., … Gellermann, W. (2013). Blue-light reflectance imaging of macular pigment in infants and children. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 54(6), 4034–4040. [PubMed]
Böhm, V., Lietz, G., Olmedilla-Alonso, B., Phelan, D., Reboul, E., Bánati, D., … Bohn, T. (2021). From carotenoid intake to carotenoid blood and tissue concentrations - implications for dietary intake recommendations. Nutrition Reviews, 79(5), 544–573. [PubMed]
Bone, R. A., Landrum, J. T., Hime, G. W., Cains, A., & Zamor, J. (1993). Stereochemistry of the human macular carotenoids. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 34(6), 2033–2040. [PubMed]
Brainard, D. H., & Hurlbert, A. C. (2015). Colour vision: Understanding #TheDress. Current Biology: CB, 25(13), R551–R554. [PubMed]
Burke, J. D., Curran-Celentano, J., & Wenzel, A. J. (2005). Diet and serum carotenoid concentrations affect macular pigment optical density in adults 45 years and older. The Journal of Nutrition, 135(5), 1208–1214. [PubMed]
Cai, J., Nelson, K. C., Wu, M., Sternberg, P., Jr., & Jones, D. P. (2000). Oxidative damage and protection of the RPE. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, 19(2), 205–221. [PubMed]
Canfield, L. M., Clandinin, M. T., Davies, D. P., Fernandez, M. C., Jackson, J., Hawkes, J., … Bo, X. (2003). Multinational study of major breast milk carotenoids of healthy mothers. European Journal of Nutrition, 42(3), 133–141. [PubMed]
Charan, J., & Biswas, T. (2013). How to calculate sample size for different study designs in medical research? Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine, 35(2), 121–126. [PubMed]
Chetverikov, A., & Ivanchei, I. (2016). Seeing “the Dress” in the right light: Perceived colors and inferred light sources. Perception, 45(8), 910–930. [PubMed]
Creutzfeldt, O. D., Crook, J. M., Kastner, S., Li, C. Y., & Pei, X. (1991). The neurophysiological correlates of colour and brightness contrast in lateral geniculate neurons. I. Population analysis. Experimental Brain Research, 87(1), 3–21. [PubMed]
Davinelli, S., Ali, S., Solfrizzi, V., Scapagnini, G., & Corbi, G. (2021). Carotenoids and cognitive outcomes: A meta-analysis of randomized intervention trials. Antioxidants (Basel, Switzerland), 10(2), 223. [PubMed]
Dacey, D. M., Liao, H. W., Peterson, B. B., Robinson, F. R., Smith, V. C., Pokorny, J., … Gamlin, P. D. (2005). Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal colour and irradiance and project to the LGN. Nature, 433(7027), 749–754. [PubMed]
Dixon, E. L., & Shapiro, A. G. (2017). Spatial filtering, color constancy, and the color-changing dress. Journal of Vision, 17(3), 7, doi:10.1167/17.3.7. [PubMed]
Do M. T. H. (2019). Melanopsin and the intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells: Biophysics to behavior. Neuron, 104(2), 205–226. [PubMed]
Drissi-Daoudi, L., Doerig, A., Parkosadze, K., Kunchulia, M., & Herzog, M. H. (2020). How stable is perception in #TheDress and #TheShoe? Vision Research, 169, 1–5. [PubMed]
Estévez-Santiago, R., Beltrán-de-Miguel, B., & Olmedilla-Alonso, B. (2016). Assessment of dietary lutein, zeaxanthin and lycopene intakes and sources in the Spanish survey of dietary intake (2009-2010). International Journal of Food Sciences and Nutrition, 67(3), 305–313. [PubMed]
Foster D. H. (2011). Color constancy. Vision Research, 51(7), 674–700. [PubMed]
Franke, S., Fröhlich, K., Werner, S., Böhm, V. & Schöne, F. (2010), Analysis of carotenoids and vitamin E in selected oilseeds, press cakes and oils. European Journal of Lipid Science and Technology, 112, 1122–1129.
González-Martín-Moro, J., Contreras, I., Fuentes-Vega, I., Prieto-Garrido, F., Castro-Rebollo, M., González-Manrique, M., … Zamora, J. (2021). Influence of main ocular variables in #TheDress perception: An ophthalmic survey. Perception, 50(4), 367–378. [PubMed]
González Martín-Moro, J., Prieto Garrido, F., Gómez Sanz, F., Fuentes Vega, I., Castro Rebollo, M., & Moreno Martín, P. (2018). Which are the colors of the dress? Review of an atypical optic illusion. ¿De qué colores es el vestido? Revisión de una ilusión óptica atípica. Archivos de la Sociedad Espanola de Oftalmologia, 93(4), 186–192. [PubMed]
Hammond, B. R., Jr., Wooten, B. R., Engles, M., & Wong, J. C. (2012). The influence of filtering by the macular carotenoids on contrast sensitivity measured under simulated blue haze conditions. Vision Research, 63, 58–62. [PubMed]
Hong, I. H., Jung, W. H., Lee, J. H., & Chang, I. B. (2020). Macular pigment optical density in the Korean population: A cross sectional study. Journal of Korean Medical Science, 35(5), e30. [PubMed]
Hurlbert, A. C. (2007). Quick guide: Colour constancy. Current Biology, 17(21), R906–R907.
Kamermans, M., Kraaij, D. A., & Spekreijse, H. (1998). The cone/horizontal cell network: A possible site for color constancy. Visual Neuroscience, 15(5), 787–797. [PubMed]
Krinsky, N. I., Landrum, J. T., & Bone, R. A. (2003). Biologic mechanisms of the protective role of lutein and zeaxanthin in the eye. Annual Review of Nutrition, 23, 171–201. [PubMed]
Lafer-Sousa, R., & Conway, B. R. (2017). #TheDress: Categorical perception of an ambiguous color image. Journal of Vision, 17(12), 25, doi:10.1167/17.12.25. [PubMed]
Lafer-Sousa, R., Hermann, K. L., & Conway, B. R. (2015). Striking individual differences in color perception uncovered by ‘the dress’ photograph. Current Biology: CB, 25(13), R545–R546. [PubMed]
Lai, J. S., Veetil, V. O., Lanca, C., Lee, B. L., Godfrey, K. M., Gluckman, P. D., … Chong, M. F. F. (2020). Maternal lutein and zeaxanthin concentrations in relation to offspring visual acuity at 3 years of age: The GUSTO Study. Nutrients, 12(2), 274. [PubMed]
Landrum, J. T., & Bone, R. A. (2001). Lutein, zeaxanthin, and the macular pigment. Archives of Biochemistry and Biophysics, 385(1), 28–40. [PubMed]
Lipkie, T. E., Morrow, A. L., Jouni, Z. E., McMahon, R. J., & Ferruzzi, M. G. (2015). Longitudinal survey of carotenoids in human milk from urban cohorts in China, Mexico, and the USA. PLoS One, 10(6), e0127729. [PubMed]
Mahmassani, H. A., Switkowski, K. M., Scott, T. M., Johnson, E. J., Rifas-Shiman, S. L., Oken, E., … Jacques, P. F. (2021). Maternal intake of lutein and zeaxanthin during pregnancy is positively associated with offspring verbal intelligence and behavior regulation in mid-childhood in the Project Viva Cohort. The Journal of Nutrition, 151(3), 615–627. [PubMed]
Mahroo, O. A., Williams, K. M., Hossain, I. T., Yonova-Doing, E., Kozareva, D., Yusuf, A., … Hammond, C. J. (2017). Do twins share the same dress code? Quantifying relative genetic and environmental contributions to subjective perceptions of “the dress” in a classical twin study. Journal of Vision, 17(1), 29, doi:10.1167/17.1.29. [PubMed]
Mewborn, C. M., Lindbergh, C. A., Robinson, T. L., Gogniat, M. A., Terry, D. P., Jean, K. R., … Miller, L. S. (2018). Lutein and zeaxanthin are positively associated with visual-spatial functioning in older adults: An fMRI study. Nutrients, 10(4), 458. [PubMed]
Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (2000). A psychophysical dissection of the brain sites involved in color-generating comparisons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(14), 8069–8074. [PubMed]
Nascimento, S. M., & Foster, D. H. (2001). Detecting changes of spatial cone-excitation ratios in dichoptic viewing. Vision Research, 41(20), 2601–2606. [PubMed]
Neumeyer, C., Dörr, S., Fritsch, J., & Kardelky, C. (2002). Colour constancy in goldfish and man: Influence of surround size and lightness. Perception, 31(2), 171–187. [PubMed]
Nolan, J. M., Beatty, S., Meagher, K. A., Howard, A. N., Kelly, D., & Thurnham, D. I. (2014). Verification of meso-zeaxanthin in fish. Journal of Food Processing & Technology, 5(6), 335. [PubMed]
Nolan, J. M., Loskutova, E., Howard, A. N., Moran, R., Mulcahy, R., Stack, J., … Beatty, S. (2014). Macular pigment, visual function, and macular disease among subjects with Alzheimer's disease: An exploratory study. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease: JAD, 42(4), 1191–1202. [PubMed]
Perry, A., Rasmussen, H., & Johnson, E. J. (2009). Xanthophyll (lutein, zeaxanthin) content in fruits, vegetables and corn and egg products. Journal of Food Composition and Analysis, 22, 9–15.
Rabin, J., Houser, B., Talbert, C., & Patel, R. (2016). Blue-black or white-gold? Early stage processing and the color of ‘The Dress’. PLoS One, 11(8), e0161090. [PubMed]
Rabin, J., Poole, E., Price, W., Kaur, G., Hall, K., Sailors, V., … Somphruek, R. (2023). A new method to quantify the human visual threshold from melanopsin sensitive ganglion cells. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 17, 1132230. [PubMed]
Randler, C., Faßl, C., & Kalb, N. (2017). From lark to owl: Developmental changes in morningness-eveningness from new-borns to early adulthood. Scientific Reports, 7, 45874. [PubMed]
Renzi-Hammond, L. M., Bovier, E. R., Fletcher, L. M., Miller, L. S., Mewborn, C. M., Lindbergh, C. A., … Hammond, B. R. (2017). Effects of a lutein and zeaxanthin intervention on cognitive function: A randomized, double-masked, placebo-controlled trial of younger healthy adults. Nutrients, 9(11), 1246. [PubMed]
Renzi, L. M., Bovier, E. R., & Hammond, B. R., Jr. (2013). A role for the macular carotenoids in visual motor response. Nutritional Neuroscience, 16(6), 262–268. [PubMed]
Retter, T. L., Gwinn, O. S., O'Neil, S. F., Jiang, F., & Webster, M. A. (2020). Neural correlates of perceptual color inferences as revealed by #thedress. Journal of Vision, 20(3), 7, doi:10.1167/jov.20.3.7. [PubMed]
Rodriguez-Amaya, D. B. (2003). Food carotenoids: Analysis, composition and alterations during storage and processing of foods. Forum of Nutrition, 56, 35–37. [PubMed]
Sasano, H., Obana, A., Sharifzadeh, M., Bernstein, P. S., Okazaki, S., Gohto, Y., … Gellermann, W. (2018). Optical detection of macular pigment formation in premature infants. Translational Vision Science & Technology, 7(4), 3. [PubMed]
Schlaffke, L., Golisch, A., Haag, L. M., Lenz, M., Heba, S., Lissek, S., … Tegenthoff, M. (2015). The brain's dress code: How The Dress allows to decode the neuronal pathway of an optical illusion. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System And Behavior, 73, 271–275. [PubMed]
Seddon, J. M., Ajani, U. A., Sperduto, R. D., Hiller, R., Blair, N., Burton, T. C., … Miller, D. T. (1994). Dietary carotenoids, vitamins A, C, and E, and advanced age-related macular degeneration. Eye Disease Case-Control Study Group. Journal of the American Medical Association, 272(18), 1413–1420.
Schein, S. J., & Desimone, R. (1990). Spectral properties of V4 neurons in the macaque. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 10(10), 3369–3389. [PubMed]
Sommerburg, O., Keunen, J. E., Bird, A. C., & van Kuijk, F. J. (1998). Fruits and vegetables that are sources for lutein and zeaxanthin: The macular pigment in human eyes. The British Journal of Ophthalmology, 82(8), 907–910. [PubMed]
Stringham, J. M., & Hammond, B. R. (2008). Macular pigment and visual performance under glare conditions. Optometry and Vision Science: Official Publication of the American Academy of Optometry, 85(2), 82–88. [PubMed]
Stringham, J. M., & Stringham, N. T. (2016). Serum and retinal responses to three different doses of macular carotenoids over 12 weeks of supplementation. Experimental Eye Research, 151, 1–8. [PubMed]
Sujak, A., Gabrielska, J., Grudziński, W., Borc, R., Mazurek, P., & Gruszecki, W. I. (1999). Lutein and zeaxanthin as protectors of lipid membranes against oxidative damage: The structural aspects. Archives of Biochemistry and Biophysics, 371(2), 301–307. [PubMed]
Thoene, M., Anderson-Berry, A., Van Ormer, M., Furtado, J., Soliman, G. A., Goldner, W., & Hanson, C. (2019). Quantification of lutein + zeaxanthin presence in human placenta and correlations with blood levels and maternal dietary intake. Nutrients, 11(1), 134. [PubMed]
van der Veen, R. L., Berendschot, T. T., Hendrikse, F., Carden, D., Makridaki, M., & Murray, I. J. (2009). A new desktop instrument for measuring macular pigment optical density based on a novel technique for setting flicker thresholds. Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics: The Journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians (Optometrists), 29(2), 127–137. [PubMed]
Vemuri, K., Bisla, K., Mulpuru, S., & Varadharajan, S. (2016). Do normal pupil diameter differences in the population underlie the color selection of #thedress? Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 33(3), A137–A142. [PubMed]
Vemuri, K., Srivastava, A., Agrawal, S., & Anand, M. (2018). Age, pupil size differences, and color choices for the “dress” and the “jacket.” Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 35(4), B347–B355. [PubMed]
Vishwanathan, R., Neuringer, M., Snodderly, D. M., Schalch, W., & Johnson, E. J. (2013). Macular lutein and zeaxanthin are related to brain lutein and zeaxanthin in primates. Nutritional Neuroscience, 16(1), 21–29. [PubMed]
Wachtler, T., Sejnowski, T. J., & Albright, T. D. (2003). Representation of color stimuli in awake macaque primary visual cortex. Neuron, 37(4), 681–691. [PubMed]
Wallisch, P. (2017). Illumination assumptions account for individual differences in the perceptual interpretation of a profoundly ambiguous stimulus in the color domain: “The dress.” Journal of Vision, 17(4), 5, doi:10.1167/17.4.5. [PubMed]
Walsh, V., Carden, D., Butler, S. R., & Kulikowski, J. J. (1993). The effects of V4 lesions on the visual abilities of macaques: Hue discrimination and colour constancy. Behavioural Brain Research, 53(1–2), 51–62. [PubMed]
Walk, A. M., Khan, N. A., Barnett, S. M., Raine, L. B., Kramer, A. F., Cohen, N. J., … Hillman, C. H. (2017). From neuro-pigments to neural efficiency: The relationship between retinal carotenoids and behavioral and neuroelectric indices of cognitive control in childhood. International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 118, 1–8. [PubMed]
Winkler, A. D., Spillmann, L., Werner, J. S., & Webster, M. A. (2015). Asymmetries in blue-yellow color perception and in the color of ‘the dress’. Current Biology: CB, 25(13), R547–R548. [PubMed]
Witzel, C., Racey, C., & O'Regan, J. K. (2017). The most reasonable explanation of “the dress”: Implicit assumptions about illumination. Journal of Vision, 17(2), 1, doi:10.1167/17.2.1. [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
Image of #TheDress. Most people perceive it either as blue/black or white/gold (https://web.archive.org/web/20150227014959/http://swiked.tumblr.com/post/112073818575/guys-please-help-me-is-this-dress-white-and). Copyright Cecilia Bleasdale.
Figure 1.
 
Image of #TheDress. Most people perceive it either as blue/black or white/gold (https://web.archive.org/web/20150227014959/http://swiked.tumblr.com/post/112073818575/guys-please-help-me-is-this-dress-white-and). Copyright Cecilia Bleasdale.
Figure 2.
 
Line graph of age distribution between groups of perception. Notice the higher perception of blue/black at 2 years and how white/gold increases through time. A statistically significant difference was found between groups (p = 0.007). Two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test: p = 0.049.
Figure 2.
 
Line graph of age distribution between groups of perception. Notice the higher perception of blue/black at 2 years and how white/gold increases through time. A statistically significant difference was found between groups (p = 0.007). Two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test: p = 0.049.
Figure 3.
 
Line graph of total carotenoid preference between groups of perception. No statistically significant difference was found when comparing the two groups (p = 0.185).
Figure 3.
 
Line graph of total carotenoid preference between groups of perception. No statistically significant difference was found when comparing the two groups (p = 0.185).
Figure 4.
 
Bar graph depicting the distribution of green-leaf preference (GLP). Notice how children who prefer to eat green leaves perceive the colors more often as white/gold (Mann–Whitney U = 19,094.0; p = 0.038), indicating a statistically significant higher GLP in the white/gold group.
Figure 4.
 
Bar graph depicting the distribution of green-leaf preference (GLP). Notice how children who prefer to eat green leaves perceive the colors more often as white/gold (Mann–Whitney U = 19,094.0; p = 0.038), indicating a statistically significant higher GLP in the white/gold group.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×