Figure 4 plots fixation stability (
Figure 4A) and pupil size (
Figure 4C) and their relationship to visual acuity (
Figures 4B and
4D). We again report a significant improvement in visual acuity following adaptation to flicker: unadapted, 0.11 ± 0.16 logMAR; adapted, 0.030 ± 0.16 logMAR; mean difference, −0.080 ± 0.060 logMAR;
t(34) = 7.78;
p < 0.001;
BF10 = 2.61 × 10
6. Note that this is a larger improvement than reported in
Experiment 1. In
Experiment 2, seven out of the 35 observers had already participated in
Experiment 1, possibly gaining more experience with the task. Moreover, each observer in
Experiment 2 conducted fewer trials (a minimum of 210 except one observer) compared with
Experiment 1 (a minimum of 420 except one observer), potentially reducing the impact of observer fatigue on the results.
Although we anticipated that flicker adaptation would improve fixation stability (lower BCEA score), we found no significant enhancement in fixation stability (larger BCEA score) following adaptation: mean difference, 0.24 ± 0.71 deg
2;
t(34) = −2.04;
p = 0.05;
BF10 = 1.13 (
Figure 4A), with the Bayes factor providing only weak support. We found no association between fixation stability and better acuity following adaptation to flicker:
r(35) = 0.04;
p = 0.84;
BF10 = 0.21 (
Figure 4B). These results suggest that fixation stability has negligible impact on the acuity gains reported here.
Observers’ pupil size was significantly reduced by adaptation to flicker. Specifically, pupil size reduced from an unadapted diameter of 5.12 ± 0.89 mm to an adapted diameter of 4.88 ± 0.92 mm. Evidence from our data strongly support the observed pupil size reduction following adaptation to flicker: mean difference, −0.25 ± 0.35 mm;
t(34) = 4.23;
p < 0.001;
BF10 = 159.68 (
Figure 4C). However, we did not find compelling evidence for a link between changes in pupil size and acuity change following adaptation to flicker:
r(35) = −0.24;
p = 0.17;
BF10 = 0.53 (
Figure 4D).
Although we anticipated flicker adaptation might increase drift curvature, our analysis did not reveal a statistically significant increase in drift curvature: unadapted, 1.45° ± 0.12°; adapted, 1.39° ± 0.21°; mean difference, −0.061° ± 0.19°; t(34) = 1.96; p = 0.058; BF10 = 1, with the Bayes factor predicting a weak effect. However, neither drift speed: unadapted, 9.97 deg/s ± 8.22 deg/s; adapted, 9.95 deg/s ± 8.07 deg/s; mean difference, −0.024 deg/s ± 4.82 deg/s; t(34) = 0.03; p = 0.98; BF10 = 0.18, nor drift distance measurements: unadapted, 31.47° ± 20.29°; adapted, 35.65° ± 32.0°; mean difference, 4.22° ± 24.95°; t(34) = −1.00; p = 0.33; BF10 = 0.29, displayed significant differences between the unadapted and flicker-adapted conditions.
Furthermore, none of these metrics displayed a strong association with changes in visual acuity: drift curvature, r(35) = −0.21, p = 0.88, BF10 = 0.42; drift speed, r(35) = −0.16, p = 0.36, BF10 = 0.32; or drift distance, r(35) = −0.24, p = 0.17, BF10 = 0.53.
On the suggestion of an anonymous reviewer, we tested our datasets for homoscedasticity, revealing that eye-movement curvature and distance data had unequal variances. As a result, we ran a two-sample t-test which does not assume equal variances, and (in agreement with the previous test) we found no statistically significant difference in curvature (p = 0.13) and distance (p = 0.56) of eye movement following adaptation to flicker.
Taken together, these results do not support our hypothesis that alterations in fixational eye movements or pupil size contribute significantly to changes in visual acuity arising from flicker adaptation.