We examined whether distractor interference was larger for distractors on the horizontal than vertical meridian. To illustrate the comparison, we show examples of target and distractor locations in
Figure 3. As shown in
Figure 3A, we compared distractors on the vertical meridian with distractors on the horizontal meridian. To keep the same distance between target and distractor, we only analyzed trials where target and distractor were on opposite meridians (i.e., horizontal distractor with vertical target and the other way around). To partial out effects of target location, we included distractor-absent trials in our analysis. Therefore, we had to run the ANOVA with target location (and not distractor location) as the factor. That is, in
Figure 3A, the
x-axis shows horizontal vs. vertical target location (HOR vs. VRT). We conducted a 2 (task: line, dot) × 2 (distractor: present, absent) × 2 (target meridian: horizontal, vertical) mixed ANOVA. The full ANOVA tables are available on OSF (RT:
https://osf.io/upkzg/; Errors rates:
https://osf.io/sbny8/). RTs were longer on distractor-present than distractor-absent trials (543 ms vs. 523 ms),
F(1, 46) = 89.59,
p < 0.001, η
p2 = 0.661. Importantly, the effect of distractor presence was modulated by target meridian,
F(1, 46) = 5.06,
p = 0.029, η
p2 = 0.099. The difference between distractor-present and distractor-absent trials was 17 ms when the distractor was shown on the vertical meridian (with HOR targets), but this difference was greater (22 ms) when the distractor was shown on the horizontal meridian (with VRT targets). Thus, we find that presenting distractors on the horizontal meridian results in stronger interference than presenting them on the vertical meridian, at least with targets on the opposite meridian. This result is consistent with the hypothesis that high-performance regions provide a bottom-up bias in the competition between target and distractor.
Further, there were some unpredicted results that do not directly speak to our hypotheses. RTs were longer in the line than in the dot task (557 ms vs. 509 ms), F(1, 46) = 9.24, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.167. The effect of target meridian, F(1, 46) = 22.48, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.328, was modulated by task, F(1, 46) = 5.28, p = 0.026, ηp2 = 0.103. In the line task, RTs were about the same for targets on the horizontal and vertical meridian (555 ms vs. 560 ms), t(23) = 1.73, p = 0.091, Cohen's dz = 0.353, whereas in the dot task, RTs were shorter for targets on the horizontal than vertical meridian (502 ms vs. 515 ms), t(23) = 4.98, p < 0.001, dz = 1.02. Further, the analysis of choice errors confirmed worse performance in the presence than absence of a distractor (5.4% vs. 3.8%), F(1, 46) = 40.47, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.468. In addition, there were more errors for targets on the horizontal than vertical meridian (5.2% vs. 4.1%), F(1, 46) = 7.87, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.146.