This study introduces a new quick method for measuring the relative strength of the contribution of the two eyes to binocular vision in the lab or clinic. With an anaglyph presentation, it can be delivered on a variety of digital devices. It involves a simple task of ranking the visibility of dichoptically presented stimuli, which after only 10 to 20 trials provides a quantitative estimate of the magnitude and direction of any imbalance along with the associated measurement error. It has excellent test–retest reliability both in the combined analysis of all of our conditions (
Figure 4A) and in the analysis of the results from individual filter or lens conditions (
Figure A1).
We chose to compare the results of the DLT (
Kwon et al., 2015) with those obtained from the DiCOT. We did not expect that the two methods would measure an identical interocular suppression process. The DLT measures overlay suppression—namely, suppression between corresponding regions of the visual field in both eyes which are presented with rivalrous stimuli. Our DiCOT, on the other hand, measures dichoptic differences in perceived contrast, which may be mediated by suppression arising from surrounding regions of the visual field from the other eye. We expected the DLT result to mostly reflect feedforward and local intercortical interactions, whereas the DiCOT was expected to depend more on feedback interactions from higher cortical areas (
Angelucci & Shushruth, 2013). However, what the tests have in common is that they both are suitable to be used for clinical measurements. They are appropriate for children as well as for adult patients, and they both provide the error associated with the measurement.
When penalizing lenses and filters were used, both tests showed changes in the binocular imbalance (
Figure 3). The DLT, which we propose is based on effects of overlay suppression and rivalry dominance (where that rivalry state itself may affect the behavior of the system; see
Klink et al., 2010), gave more extreme values than the DiCOT. Our DiCOT measure is designed to reflect both the feed-forward contrast gain of the inputs of the two eyes (without local competition), as well as any effects of dichoptic surround suppression from the elements seen in the other eye. Both tests exhibited excellent test–retest reliability. Although the pattern of results found with the two tests was similar, the results from one accounted for around a third of the variance in the results from the other (
Figure 5). We argue that they target (partially) distinct aspects of binocular vision and so are differentially affected by our different lens and filter condition manipulations. This is reflected by the different regression slopes reported in
Table 2. For example, to the extent that the DLT is a rivalry-based task, there is an expectation that a blurred input in one eye (from our +3-D lens) would greatly reduce the dominance of that blurred image (
Arnold, Grove, & Wallis, 2007). On the other hand, we would also expect a (less dramatic) reduction in the perceived contrast in the DiCOT if the image were blurred in one eye (
May & Georgeson, 2007). This exaggerated effect of blurring on the DLT is reflected visually in our results (
Figure 3) and by the relatively shallow regression slope (
Table 2). Determining the best instructions and training to separate blur from contrast judgments in the DiCOT is a topic for future research. If we adjusted the data from each filter or lens condition individually, we did find they each showed a comparable consistency between the DiCOT and the DLT (consistency ICC scores of 0.56–0.63; see
Table 2). This is still lower than the test–retest reliability, indicating a residual difference between the two measurements. The possibility that the DiCOT and the DLT may target distinct aspects of binocular imbalance makes the combined use of both tests interesting. One such study has been performed comparing the two measurement tools in amblyopia (
Reynaud et al., 2024).
Although both the DLT and the DiCOT should reflect imbalances in the gain of the input from each eye, we found (as mentioned above) a wider range in the DLT results. This may be due to the rivalrous stimulus design. Exaggeration of the imbalance for measurements made with rivalrous stimuli would also be expected in amblyopia (
Meier, Tarczy-Hornoch, Boynton, & Fine, 2023). The other difference of interest to us was the ways in which the two tasks would be affected by interocular suppression. Spatially, suppression can be induced by both stimuli occupying a corresponding retinal locus (overlay suppression) and those in neighboring regions of the visual field (surround suppression). Overlay suppression is thought to mostly involve local responses from neurons responding within the stimulus area and exhibits orientation tuning. Surround suppression involves more remote suppressive interactions. These involve both intracortical horizontal connections and likely feedback contributions from higher visual areas (
Angelucci & Shushruth, 2013). Very remote surround interactions do not exhibit orientational tuning (
Shushruth et al., 2013). Interocular suppression in individuals with normal healthy vision that have induced interocular imbalances or in amblyopic individuals who have intrinsic interocular imbalances as a result of a disruption of normal visual development involves contributions of both overlay and surround suppression (
Huang, Zhou, Lu, Feng, & Zhou, 2012). This imbalance in amblyopia does not affect behavior on contrast matching tasks when stimuli are presented monocularly (
Hess & Bradley, 1980), suggesting that there is no suprathreshold impact of interocular suppression on perceived contrast under monocular stimulation. Under dichoptic viewing, however, contrast matching is impacted (
Reynaud & Hess, 2016). The specificity of this phenomenon to simultaneous dichoptic presentation underlies our argument that imbalances measured with the DiCOT can be considered to reflect interocular suppression. However, for the many purposes for which this tool may be used, the distinction between imbalances in gain and those in suppression may be irrelevant.