November 2024
Volume 24, Issue 12
Open Access
Article  |   November 2024
How the window of visibility varies around polar angle
Author Affiliations
  • Yuna Kwak
    Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA
    yk2191@nyu.edu
  • Zhong-Lin Lu
    Division of Arts & Sciences, New York University Shanghai, Shanghai, China
    NYU-ECNU Institute of Brain and Cognitive Science, Shanghai, China
    Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, USA
    zhonglin@nyu.edu
  • Marisa Carrasco
    Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA
    Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, USA
    marisa.carrasco@nyu.edu
Journal of Vision November 2024, Vol.24, 4. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.12.4
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Yuna Kwak, Zhong-Lin Lu, Marisa Carrasco; How the window of visibility varies around polar angle. Journal of Vision 2024;24(12):4. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.12.4.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Contrast sensitivity, the amount of contrast required to discriminate an object, depends on spatial frequency (SF). The contrast sensitivity function (CSF) peaks at intermediate SFs and drops at other SFs. The CSF varies from foveal to peripheral vision, but only a couple of studies have assessed how the CSF changes with polar angle of the visual field. For many visual dimensions, sensitivity is better along the horizontal than the vertical meridian and at the lower than the upper vertical meridian, yielding polar angle asymmetries. Here, for the first time, to our knowledge, we investigate CSF attributes around polar angle at both group and individual levels and examine the relations in CSFs across locations and individual observers. To do so, we used hierarchical Bayesian modeling, which enables precise estimation of CSF parameters. At the group level, maximum contrast sensitivity and the SF at which the sensitivity peaks are higher at the horizontal than vertical meridian and at the lower than the upper vertical meridian. By analyzing the covariance across observers (n = 28), we found that, at the individual level, CSF attributes (e.g., maximum sensitivity) across locations are highly correlated. This correlation indicates that, although the CSFs differ across locations, the CSF at one location is predictive of that at another location. Within each location, the CSF attributes covary, indicating that CSFs across individuals vary in a consistent manner (e.g., as maximum sensitivity increases, so does the corresponding SF), but more so at the horizontal than the vertical meridian locations. These results show similarities and uncover some critical polar angle differences across locations and individuals, suggesting that the CSF should not be generalized across isoeccentric locations around the visual field.

Introduction
Contrast sensitivity, the ability to discriminate visual patterns from a uniform background, is closely related to performance in daily tasks, such as face recognition (Wolffsohn, Eperjesi, & Napper, 2005), reading (Brussee, van den Berg, van Nispen, & van Rens, 2017; Rubin & Legge, 1989), mobility (Marron & Bailey, 1982), and driving (Owsley & McGwin, 2010; Schieber, Kline, Kline, & Fozard, 1992). It is also central for clinical assessment (Owsley, 2003; Pelli & Bex, 2013), as contrast sensitivity is impaired in ophthalmic conditions such as myopia (Anders et al., 2023; Baldwin et al., 2023; Collins & Carney, 1990; Ginsburg, 2006; but see Xu et al., 2022) and amblyopia (Hou et al., 2010; Levi & Li, 2009; Zhou et al., 2006), and in neurological conditions such as cerebral lesions (Bodis-Wollner, 1972) and schizophrenia (Cimmer et al., 2006). 
Contrast sensitivity varies substantially as a function of spatial frequency (SF). The contrast sensitivity function (CSF) characterizes an individual's ability to reliably detect stimuli with varying SFs at a location and is typically bandpass, peaking at mid-SFs (Figure 1A). It is considered the window of visibility because objects with properties within the CSF-specified region are visible (Campbell & Robson, 1968; Watson, 2013; Watson, Ahumada, & Farrell, 1986). The CSF has also served as the front-end filter in observer models of object recognition in which the CSF controls the gain for different spatial frequency components of objects (Chung, Legge, & Tjan, 2002; Hou, Lu, & Huang, 2014; Majaj, Pelli, Kurshan, & Palomares, 2002; Rovamo, Luntinen, & Näsänen, 1993; Watson & Ahumada, 2005). Because the CSF provides critical information about spatial vision, efforts have been made to model how it varies with different stimulus conditions (Van Surdam Graham, 1989; Watson, 2018), including luminance (e.g., Rovamo, Virsu, & Näsänen, 1978), size (e.g., Rovamo et al., 1993), stimulus duration (e.g., Legge, 1978), temporal characteristics (e.g., Kelly, 1979; Robson, 1966), and eccentricity (e.g., Hilz & Cavonius, 1974; Rovamo et al., 1978). For example, at farther eccentricities, the CSF shifts downward and toward lower SFs, reducing the window of visibility (Hilz & Cavonius, 1974; Jigo & Carrasco, 2020; Rovamo et al., 1978; Virsu & Rovamo, 1979). 
Figure 1.
 
Experimental schematics and procedure. (A) CSF analysis. The CSF depicts how contrast sensitivity depends on SF. The inverse of the contrast threshold (c–1) is contrast sensitivity (S) at a particular SF. Bayesian inference was used to estimate the CSF parameters: peak-CS (maximum sensitivity), peak-SF (the most preferred SF), and bandwidth (closely related to CSF shape). Bandwidth was fixed across locations (see Methods). Results of the cutoff-SF (highest discernable SF) and AULCSF (total sensitivity) are reported in Supplementary Appendix. Higher values of these attributes are related to higher sensitivity, better performance, and/or a wider range of visible SFs and contrasts. (B) Polar angle asymmetries. In many visual dimensions, performance is higher at the horizontal than the vertical meridian (HVA) and at the lower than the upper vertical meridian (VMA). (C) Task. A test Gabor stimulus was presented briefly at one of the four polar angle locations, and participants judged its orientation (clockwise or counterclockwise). SF and contrast of the stimulus varied on each trial. (D) Schematics of covariance analysis. The covariance of the three CSF attributes (peak-CS, peak-SF, bandwidth) for all three locations is a 9 × 9 matrix. The covariation of locations for each attribute (blue cells) and the covariation of attributes within each location (pink cells) were assessed. Covariance for bandwidth is shaded in gray because the bandwidth was identical across locations for each observer in the HBM and, thus, the covariance was not informative in most cases (but see Figure 4). (E) Interpretation of covariance analysis. Covariance of locations for each attribute (D, blue cells) tests whether CSFs and their corresponding key attributes covary as a function of polar angle. For example, a perfect positive correlation (coefficient of 1) for peak-CS among locations X and Y indicates that an observer with a higher peak-CS at location X than other individuals also has a higher peak-CS at location Y (E, blue panel, top). This relation does not hold for a scenario where there is no correlation (coefficient of 0) for any CSF attributes between locations X and Y. Covariance within each location (D, pink cells) allows investigation of how individuals’ CSFs relate to one another within each location. A perfect positive covariation (correlation coefficient of 1) of all combinations of attributes within a location indicates that observers’ CSFs are organized diagonally in the SF-contrast space: The higher the peak-CS, the higher the peak-SF and the wider the bandwidth (E, pink panel, top). The fewer the correlations, the greater the variability in the pattern of individual differences (E, pink panel, bottom, is a scenario where no correlations are present among CSF attributes within a location).
Figure 1.
 
Experimental schematics and procedure. (A) CSF analysis. The CSF depicts how contrast sensitivity depends on SF. The inverse of the contrast threshold (c–1) is contrast sensitivity (S) at a particular SF. Bayesian inference was used to estimate the CSF parameters: peak-CS (maximum sensitivity), peak-SF (the most preferred SF), and bandwidth (closely related to CSF shape). Bandwidth was fixed across locations (see Methods). Results of the cutoff-SF (highest discernable SF) and AULCSF (total sensitivity) are reported in Supplementary Appendix. Higher values of these attributes are related to higher sensitivity, better performance, and/or a wider range of visible SFs and contrasts. (B) Polar angle asymmetries. In many visual dimensions, performance is higher at the horizontal than the vertical meridian (HVA) and at the lower than the upper vertical meridian (VMA). (C) Task. A test Gabor stimulus was presented briefly at one of the four polar angle locations, and participants judged its orientation (clockwise or counterclockwise). SF and contrast of the stimulus varied on each trial. (D) Schematics of covariance analysis. The covariance of the three CSF attributes (peak-CS, peak-SF, bandwidth) for all three locations is a 9 × 9 matrix. The covariation of locations for each attribute (blue cells) and the covariation of attributes within each location (pink cells) were assessed. Covariance for bandwidth is shaded in gray because the bandwidth was identical across locations for each observer in the HBM and, thus, the covariance was not informative in most cases (but see Figure 4). (E) Interpretation of covariance analysis. Covariance of locations for each attribute (D, blue cells) tests whether CSFs and their corresponding key attributes covary as a function of polar angle. For example, a perfect positive correlation (coefficient of 1) for peak-CS among locations X and Y indicates that an observer with a higher peak-CS at location X than other individuals also has a higher peak-CS at location Y (E, blue panel, top). This relation does not hold for a scenario where there is no correlation (coefficient of 0) for any CSF attributes between locations X and Y. Covariance within each location (D, pink cells) allows investigation of how individuals’ CSFs relate to one another within each location. A perfect positive covariation (correlation coefficient of 1) of all combinations of attributes within a location indicates that observers’ CSFs are organized diagonally in the SF-contrast space: The higher the peak-CS, the higher the peak-SF and the wider the bandwidth (E, pink panel, top). The fewer the correlations, the greater the variability in the pattern of individual differences (E, pink panel, bottom, is a scenario where no correlations are present among CSF attributes within a location).
Most studies in vision have assessed visual performance only along the horizontal meridian, but visual information in the real world is distributed around the entire visual field. Behavioral sensitivity in many visual dimensions varies around polar angle (Figure 1B) (for a review, see Himmelberg et al., 2023). When eccentricity is constant, contrast sensitivity at a particular SF is better along the horizontal than the vertical meridian (horizontal–vertical anisotropy [HVA]) and better at the lower than the upper vertical meridian (vertical meridian asymmetry [VMA]), indicating polar angle asymmetries (Abrams, Nizam, & Carrasco, 2012; Baldwin, Meese, & Baker, 2012; Cameron, Tai, & Carrasco, 2002; Carrasco, Talgar, & Cameron, 2001; Himmelberg, Winawer, & Carrasco, 2020). Moreover, cortical surface area (Benson, Kupers, Barbot, Carrasco, & Winawer, 2021; Himmelberg et al., 2021; Himmelberg et al., 2023; Himmelberg, Winawer, & Carrasco, 2022), and the distribution of cone photoreceptors and retinal ganglion cells (Curcio & Allen, 1990; Webb & Kaas, 1976) vary as a function of polar angle. These differences indicate that measurements along the horizontal meridian do not generalize to other locations. However, scarce attention has been paid to characterizing the CSF at isoeccentric locations around the visual field. Only two studies have systematically assessed the entire CSF at polar angle meridians and showed that the these asymmetries are present for contrast sensitivity across a wide range of SFs (Jigo, Tavdy, Himmelberg, & Carrasco, 2023; Kwak, Zhao, Lu, Hanning, & Carrasco, 2024). Interestingly, M scaling—scaling the stimulus to equate its V1 cortical representation at different locations (Rovamo & Virsu, 1979)—eliminates eccentricity, but not polar angle differences in CSF (Cameron et al., 2002; Carrasco et al., 2001; Himmelberg et al., 2020; McAnany & Levine, 2007). In addition, behavioral asymmetries around polar angle are intensified at higher SFs (Cameron et al., 2002; Carrasco et al., 2001; Himmelberg et al., 2020; McAnany & Levine, 2007), underscoring the need to characterize the entire CSF. Here, we conducted a systematic evaluation of the CSF around polar angle by investigating key attributes at each location (Figure 1A). Importantly, for the first time, to our knowledge, we examined the relations in CSFs across locations and individual observers. We concentrated on the locations along the horizontal and vertical meridians at which differences are more likely to emerge. The intercardinal locations, at which performance does not systematically vary, exhibits intermediate performance between the horizontal and vertical meridians (e.g., Cameron et al., 2002; Carrasco et al., 2001), and the upper–lower visual field asymmetry gradually decreases with angular distance from the vertical meridian (Abrams et al., 2012; Baldwin et al., 2012; Barbot, Xue, & Carrasco, 2021). 
Given the prominence of the CSF as a building block of spatial vision, obtaining accurate CSF estimates is essential (Glassman et al., 2024; Hou et al., 2010; Lesmes, Lu, Baek, & Albright, 2010). We extracted key CSF attributes at the upper vertical, lower vertical, and horizontal meridian (Figures 1A and 1C), using hierarchical Bayesian modeling (HBM; see Methods and Supplementary Figure A1), which enables accurate and precise CSF estimates with relatively few observations (Zhao, Lesmes, Hou, & Lu, 2021). Peak-CS is the maximum contrast sensitivity, peak-SF is the SF corresponding to the peak-CS and thus the most preferred SF, and bandwidth is the width of the CSF. See the Supplementary Appendix for cutoff-SF, the highest perceivable SF, and the area under the log CSF (AULCSF), or total sensitivity. We found that across polar angle locations, the shape of the CSF is constant, but that sensitivity (e.g., peak-CS and peak-SF) is higher at the horizontal than the vertical (HVA), and at the lower than the upper vertical meridian (VMA), consistent with the only two studies that have extracted and compared CSF attributes around polar angle (Jigo et al., 2023; Kwak et al., 2024), both from our group. 
Importantly, to examine the relations in CSFs across locations and across individual observers, we analyzed the covariance of the CSF attributes (Figures 1D and 1E). First, across locations, CSF attributes were correlated (e.g., peak-CS at horizontal and peak-CS at upper vertical meridian), indicating that an observer with an enhanced CSF (e.g., higher contrast sensitivity) at one location also had enhanced CSFs at other locations compared to other observers (for a possible scenario, see Figure 1E, blue panel, top). Second, within each location, CSF attributes were highly correlated with one another (e.g., peak-CS at horizontal and peak-SF at horizontal meridian), indicating that individuals’ CSFs differ in a systematic manner (for a possible scenario, see Figure 1E, pink panel, top), especially at the horizontal meridian. Observers’ CSFs are shifted along the diagonal direction in the log(SF)–log(contrast sensitivity) space. This study reveals critical differences in and relations among CSFs around polar angle, at both group and individual levels, indicating that CSFs should not be generalized around the meridians. 
Methods
Participants
Twenty-eight observers (14 males and 14 females, including author YK; ages 21–34 years) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated in the experiment. All participants except for the first author were naïve to the experimental hypothesis. The experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board at New York University, and all participants provided informed consent. They were paid $12 per hour. All procedures adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Data for seven out of the 28 participants have been reported in a previous study (Kwak et al., 2024). 
Setup
Participants sat in a dark room with their head stabilized by a chin and forehead rest. All stimuli were generated and presented using MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) and the Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997) on a gamma-linearized 20-inch ViewSonic G220fb CRT screen (Brea, CA, USA) at a viewing distance of 79 cm. The CRT screen had a resolution of 1280 × 960 pixels and a refresh rate of 100 Hz. Gaze position was recorded using an EyeLink 1000 Desktop Mount eye tracker (SR Research, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada) at a sampling rate of 1 kHz. The EyeLink Toolbox was used for eye tracking with MATLAB and Psychophysics Toolbox (Cornelissen, Peters, & Palmer, 2002). 
Experimental procedure
Participants performed an orientation discrimination task for stimuli varying in contrast and spatial frequency. Figure 1C shows the trial sequence. Each trial started with a fixation circle (0.175° radius) on a gray background (∼26 cd/m2) with a duration randomly jittered between 400 ms and 600 ms. Four placeholders indicated the locations of the upcoming stimuli, 6° left, right, above, and below fixation. Measurements at the left and right locations were combined for analysis, as it is well established that contrast sensitivity does not differ between these locations (Cameron et al., 2002). We focused on the cardinal locations because visual performance does not systematically differ along the intercardinal locations (e.g., Cameron et al., 2002; Carrasco et al., 2001), and the upper–lower visual field asymmetry gradually decreases with angular distance from the vertical meridian (Abrams et al., 2012; Baldwin et al., 2012; Barbot et al., 2021). Each placeholder was composed of four corners (black lines, 0.2° length). The trial began once a 300-ms stable fixation (eye coordinates within a 1.75° radius virtual circle centered on fixation) was detected. After detection of stable fixation, a non-informative cue (four black lines) appeared (each, 0.35° length) pointing to all locations. Then, 140 ms after the onset of the cue, a test Gabor grating (tilted ±45 relative to vertical, random phase, delimited by a raised cosine envelope with 2° radius) appeared for 30 ms at one of the four locations. The spatial frequency and the contrast of the Gabor stimulus were determined by the quantitative CSF (qCSF) procedure on each trial, based on the maximum expected information gain (Lesmes et al., 2010). Then, 450 ms after stimulus offset, the location at which the test Gabor was presented was highlighted by increasing the width and length of the corresponding placeholders. The non-informative cue pointing to all directions changed to a response cue, which was a black line pointing to the test Gabor location. Participants performed an unspeeded, orientation discrimination task for the Gabor grating (by pressing the left arrow key for –45° and the right arrow key for +45°). 
Participants were instructed to maintain fixation throughout the entire trial sequence. Gaze position was monitored online to ensure fixation within a 1.75°-radius virtual circle from the central fixation until the response phase (fixation blocks) or until cue onset (saccade blocks). Trials in which gaze deviated from fixation were aborted and repeated at the end of each block. 
To extract the CSF, we measured contrast thresholds at various spatial frequencies (Figure 1A). To place trials efficiently in the dynamic range of the CSF, we leveraged the qCSF procedure for data collection (Lesmes et al., 2010). Given the participants’ performance in previous trials, this procedure selects the stimulus contrast and spatial frequency to be tested on each trial, based on the maximum expected information gain (reduction in entropy), to further refine the CSF parameter estimates. The possible stimulus space was composed of 60 contrast levels from 0.001 to 1 and 12 spatial frequency levels from 0.5 to 16 cpd, evenly spaced in log units. There is a large body of work on qCSF validation that compares it with the ψ method (Hou, Lesmes, Bex, Dorr, & Lu, 2015; Lesmes et al., 2010) and evaluates its test–retest reliability (Chen et al., 2021; Finn et al., 2024; Thurman, Davey, McCray, Paronian, & Seitz, 2016). Although the qCSF is more efficient, its precision and outputs have been shown to be very similar to measuring the entire CSF with conventional psychophysical methods. 
CSF analysis
Bayesian inference procedure
To characterize the CSFs for each location (upper, lower, horizontal), we used the Bayesian inference procedure (BIP) (Supplementary Figure A1a) and HBM (Supplementary Figure A1b) to fit the three-parameter CSF model to trial-by-trial data points (Zhao, Lesmes, Hou et al., 2021). For both the BIP and the HBM, Bayesian inference was used to estimate the posterior distribution of the three CSF parameters: peak-CS, peak-SF, and bandwidth. Other key attributes, such as the cutoff-SF and AULCSF, were computed from the final estimate of the CSF. Because cutoff-SF and AULCSF are dependent on the three CSF parameters, we report results of these attributes in the Supplementary Appendix
Contrast sensitivity at SF is modeled as a log-parabola function with parameters θ = (peak-CS, peak-SF, bandwidth) (Lesmes et al., 2010; Watson & Ahumada, 2005):  
\begin{eqnarray} && log_{{10}}\left( {S\left( {sf,\theta } \right)} \right) = log_{{10}}\left( {peakCS} \right)\nonumber\\ && \quad - \frac{4}{{log_{{10}}\left( 2 \right)}}{\left( {\frac{{log_{{10}}\left( {sf} \right) - log_{{10}}\left( {peakSF} \right)}}{{bandwidth}}} \right)^2}. \quad \end{eqnarray}
(1)
 
The probability of a correct response (r = 1) on a trial given the stimulus—spatial frequency sf and contrast c—is described as a psychometric function (Figure 1A):  
\begin{eqnarray} && p(r = 1\ |\theta , sf,c) = g\nonumber\\ && \quad + \left( {1 - g - \lambda /2} \right) \left( {1 - {\rm{exp}}\left( { - {{\left( {cS\left( {sf,\theta } \right)} \right)}^\beta }} \right)} \right),\quad \end{eqnarray}
(2)
where g is the guessing rate (g = 0.5), λ is the lapse rate (λ = 0.04) (Wichmann & Hill, 2001; Zhao, Lesmes, Hou, et al., 2021), and β determines the slope of the psychometric function (β = 2). The probability of making an incorrect response (r = 0) is  
\begin{eqnarray} p\left( {r = 0\ {\rm{|}}\theta , sf,c} \right) = 1 - p\ \left( {r = 1\ {\rm{|}}\theta , sf,c} \right). \quad \end{eqnarray}
(3)
 
Equations 2 and 3 define the likelihood function, or the probability of a correct/incorrect response in a trial given the stimulus and the CSF parameters. To infer the CSF parameters given the experimental data, Bayes’ rule is used to estimate the posterior distribution of the CSF parameters (θ) based on the likelihood and the prior (Equation 7) in the BIP procedure. Details on modeling the prior distributions can be found below (Equations 8 to 10). 
HBM: Three-level hierarchy
The BIP fits these parameters independently for each individual and condition (Supplementary Figure A1a). Although the BIP has been proven to be a good estimate of the CSF, it may have overestimated the variance of each test because it scores each test independently with a uniform prior without considering potential relations of the parameters (Zhao, Lesmes, Dorr, & Lu, 2021). Therefore, we leveraged a hierarchical model that recently has been shown to reduce the uncertainties of the parameter estimates when fitting the CSF (Zhao, Lesmes, Dorr et al., 2021; Zhao, Lesmes, Hou et al., 2021). We used a three-level HBM with the same structure as in Zhao, Lesmes, Dorr et al. (2021) (Supplementary Figure A1b). In the current study, there were 28 individuals (participants; I = 28) and three conditions (locations; J = 3), and all trials that each participant completed were combined into one test (K = 1). Note that, in our previous work utilizing similar procedures (Kwak et al., 2024), we used a simpler three-level HBM that did not take into consideration the different conditions in the model structure (Zhao, Lesmes, Dorr et al., 2021). 
The HBM considers potential relations of the CSF parameters and hyperparameters within and across hierarchies. More specifically, it quantifies the joint distribution of the CSF parameters and hyperparameters at three hierarchies in a single model: test level (K), individual level (I), and population level. The within-individual and cross-individual regularities across experimental conditions (1:J) are modeled as the covariance of the CSF parameters at the individual level (φij) and covariance at the population level (Σ), respectively. The model incorporates conditional dependencies: CSF parameters at the test level are conditionally dependent on hyperparameters at the individual level, and the CSF hyperparameters at the individual level are conditionally dependent on those at the population level. Incorporating this knowledge into the model and decomposing the variability of the entire dataset into distributions at multiple hierarchies enabled us to reduce the variance of the test-level estimates and to obtain more precise estimates of the CSF parameters. 
We fit the BIP and the HBM sequentially. As a first step, the BIP was fit to obtain the mean and standard deviation of each of the three CSF parameters across participants for each condition, using a uniform prior distribution. Next, these values were used to set the prior for HBM (see Equations 8 to 10). 
At the population level of the HBM, the joint distribution of hyperparameter η across J experimental conditions was modeled as a mixture of three-dimensional Gaussian distributions N with mean µ and covariance Σ, which have distributions p(µ) and p(Σ):  
\begin{eqnarray} p\left( \eta \right) = N\left( {\eta , \mu , \Sigma } \right)\ p\left( \mu \right)p\left( \Sigma \right).\quad \end{eqnarray}
(4)
 
At the individual level (I), the joint distribution of hyperparameter τi,1:J of individual i across all the 1:J experimental conditions was modeled as a mixture of three-dimensional Gaussian distributions N with mean ρij and covariance φj, which have distributions pi,1:J|η) and pj), where pi,1:J|η) denotes that the mean was conditioned on the population-level hyperparameter:  
\begin{eqnarray} && p({\tau _{i,1:J}}|\eta )\nonumber\\ && \quad = p\left( {{\rho _{i,1:J}}{\rm{|}}\eta } \right)\mathop \prod \limits_{j = 1}^J N\left( {{\tau _{ij}}, {\rho _{ij}},{\varphi _j}} \right) p\left( {{\varphi _j}} \right).\quad \end{eqnarray}
(5)
 
At the test level (K), pijkij), the joint distribution of parameter θijk of individual i, condition j, and test k was conditioned on hyperparameter τij at the individual level. 
The probability of obtaining the entire dataset Y was computed by probability multiplication:  
\begin{eqnarray} && p({Y_{1:I, 1:J, 1:K,1:M}}|X)\nonumber\\ &&\quad = \mathop \prod \limits_{i = 1}^I \mathop \prod \limits_{j = 1}^J \mathop \prod \limits_{k = 1}^K \mathop \prod \limits_{m = 1}^M p({r_{ijkm}}|{\theta _{ijk}}, s{f_{ijkm}},{c_{ijkm}})\nonumber\\ &&\qquad\times\, p({\theta _{ijk}}|{\tau _{ij}}) p({\tau _{i,1:J}}|\eta )\ p\left( \eta \right)\nonumber\\ && \quad = \mathop \prod \limits_{i = 1}^I \mathop \prod \limits_{j = 1}^J \mathop \prod \limits_{k = 1}^K \mathop \prod \limits_{m = 1}^M p({r_{ijkm}}|{\theta _{ijk}}, s{f_{ijkm}},{c_{ijkm}})\nonumber\\ &&\qquad\times\, p({\theta _{ijk}}|{\tau _{ij}}) N\left( {{\tau _{ij}},{\rho _{ij}},{\varphi _j}} \right)p\left( {{\varphi _j}} \right) p({\rho _{i,1:J}}|\eta )\nonumber\\ &&\qquad\times\, N\left( {\eta , \mu , \Sigma } \right)\, p\left( \mu \right)\ p\left( \Sigma \right) , \quad \end{eqnarray}
(6)
where X = (θ1:I,1:J,1:K, ρ1:I,1:J, φ1:J, µ, Σ) are all parameters and hyperparameters in the HBM, rijkm is a response on a given trial, and sfijkm and cijkm are spatial frequency and contrast, respectively, of the stimulus on a given trial. 
HBM: Computing the joint posterior distribution
Bayes’ rule \((posterior\ probability\ p(A|B) = \frac{{likelihood\ p(B|A) \times prior\ probability\ p( A )}}{{marginal\ {\mathit{probability}}\ p( B )}})\) was used to compute the joint posterior distribution of all the parameters and hyperparameters in the HBM:  
\begin{eqnarray} && p(X|{Y_{1:I, 1:J, 1:K,1:M}})\nonumber\\ && = \frac{\begin{array}{@{}l@{}} \mathop \prod \nolimits_{i = 1}^I \mathop \prod \nolimits_{j = 1}^J \mathop \prod \nolimits_{k = 1}^K \mathop \prod \nolimits_{m = 1}^M p({r_{ijkm}}|{\theta _{ijk}}, s{f_{ijkm}},{c_{ijkm}})\\ \quad\times\, p({\theta _{ijk}}|{\tau _{ij}})N\left( {{\tau _{ij}},{\rho _{ij}},{\varphi _j}} \right){p_0}\left( {{\varphi _j}} \right)\\ \quad\times\, p({\rho _{i,1:J}}|\eta )N\left( {\eta , \mu , \Sigma } \right)\ {p_0}\left( \mu \right)\ {p_0}\left( \Sigma \right)\end{array}} {\begin{array}{@{}l@{}}\int\mathop \prod \nolimits_{i = 1}^I \mathop \prod \nolimits_{j = 1}^J \mathop \prod \nolimits_{k = 1}^K \mathop \prod \nolimits_{m = 1}^M p({r_{ijkm}}|{\theta _{ijk}}, s{f_{ijkm}},{c_{ijkm}})\\ \quad\times\, p({\theta _{ijk}}|{\tau _{ij}})N\left( {{\tau _{ij}},{\rho _{ij}},{\varphi _j}} \right){p_0}\left( {{\varphi _j}} \right)\ p({\rho _{i,1:J}}|\eta )\\ \quad\times\,N\left( {\eta , \mu , \Sigma } \right)\ {p_0}\left( \mu \right)\ {p_0}\left( \Sigma \right)dX\end{array}},\nonumber\\ \end{eqnarray}
(7)
where the denominator is the probability of obtaining the entire dataset (Equation 6). 
We used the JAGS package in R (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) to evaluate the joint posterior distribution. JAGS generates representative samples of the joint posterior distribution of all the parameters and hyperparameters in the HBM via Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC). We ran three parallel MCMC chains, each generating 2000 samples, resulting in a total of 6000 samples. Steps in the burn-in and adaptation phases—20,000 and 500,000 steps, respectively—were discarded and excluded from the analysis because the initial part of the random walk process is largely determined by random starting values. 
Prior distributions in the HBM
For fitting the HBM, we started with prior distributions of µ (population mean), Σ–1 (inverse of population level covariance matrix), and φ–1 (inverse of individual-level covariance matrix), which are p0(µ), p0–1), and p0–1), respectively. 
For each of the CSF parameters, the prior distribution of µ is a uniform distribution:  
\begin{eqnarray} {p_0}( {{\mu _{peakCS}}} ) = U( {log_{{10}}( {10} ), log_{{10}} ( {200} )} )\quad \end{eqnarray}
(8a)
 
\begin{eqnarray} {p_0} ( {log_{{10}}({\mu _{peakSF}})}) = U( {log_{{10}}( {0.31} ),log_{{10}}( {3.16} )} )\quad \end{eqnarray}
(8b)
 
\begin{eqnarray} {p_0}( {log_{{10}}({\mu _{bandwidth}})} ) = U( {log_{{10}}( {1.58}),log_{{10}} ( {3.98} )} )\quad \end{eqnarray}
(8c)
 
The prior distributions of the precision matrices (the inverse of covariance matrices Σ and φ) are modeled as Wishart distributions. W(Y,v) denotes a Wishart distribution with expected precision matrix Y and degrees of freedom v (where v = 4). ΣBIP and φBIP are population-level and individual-level covariance matrices obtained from the BIP fit:  
\begin{eqnarray} {p_0}({\Sigma ^{ - 1}}) = W\left( {{\Sigma _{BIP}}^{ - 1},v} \right),\quad \end{eqnarray}
(9)
 
\begin{eqnarray} {p_0}({\varphi ^{ - 1}}) = W\left( {{\varphi _{BIP}}^{ - 1},v} \right).\quad \end{eqnarray}
(10)
 
Model comparison
To directly test whether the shape of the CSF differs around the polar angle, we compared two models: one in which bandwidth was fixed across locations (fixed bandwidth model) for each observer, and one in which bandwidth was free to vary across locations (varying bandwidth model) for each observer. In the fixed bandwidth model, there were seven parameters per participant: 2 parameters (peak-CS, peak-SF) × 3 locations + 1 parameter (bandwidth). In the varying bandwidth model, there were nine parameters per participant: 3 parameters (peak-CS, peak-SF, bandwidth) × 3 locations. Here, we report results from the fixed bandwidth model, which is more parsimonious. That the current study used a fixed bandwidth model explains why the patterns in peak-SF (Figure 2C) and bandwidth across polar angle are not identical to those in prior work using a varying bandwidth model (Jigo et al., 2023; Kwak et al., 2024). 
Figure 2.
 
Polar angle sensitivity differences. (A) CSFs. An example participant's data are shown in Supplementary Figure A2. (B) Peak contrast sensitivity. (C) Peak spatial frequency. Results for cutoff-SF and AULCSF are shown in Supplementary Figure A3. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Error bars are ±1 SEM. HVA and VMA denote horizontal-vertical anisotropy and vertical meridian asymmetries, respectively.
Figure 2.
 
Polar angle sensitivity differences. (A) CSFs. An example participant's data are shown in Supplementary Figure A2. (B) Peak contrast sensitivity. (C) Peak spatial frequency. Results for cutoff-SF and AULCSF are shown in Supplementary Figure A3. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Error bars are ±1 SEM. HVA and VMA denote horizontal-vertical anisotropy and vertical meridian asymmetries, respectively.
Covariance matrix
The 15 × 15 covariance matrix across all attributes and locations (5 attributes × 3 locations = 15 combinations) from the HBM outputs was obtained by computing the covariance of a 28 × 15 matrix (28 participants, 15 combinations). Shrinkage estimates of the covariance matrices were computed with the R package corpcor. The correlation matrix of the covariance matrix was obtained with the MATLAB function corrcov, which returns the Pearson correlation coefficients. The confidence intervals for the correlation coefficients were computed from the distribution of the MCMC samples (Supplementary Figure A7). Note that only the 9 × 9 matrix (3 attributes × 3 locations = 9 combinations) is presented in the main text, and other attributes are reported in the Supplementary Appendix. In addition, we report the covariance matrix from BIP outputs to demonstrate that the correlations we observed are not an artifact of the HBM. The correlation coefficients from the BIP and HBM were qualitatively similar (Supplementary Figures A8 and A9), and the order of the correlation coefficients was preserved across BIP and HBM. Using HBM outputs resulted in a larger number of significant correlations, given its improved statistical power obtained from incorporating the hierarchical structure (see HBM: Three-level hierarchy section). We also report the variation across individuals (coefficient of variation) for each CSF attribute to compare the variation for each attribute that has different units (Supplementary Table A1). 
Statistical analysis
Convergence of the HBM parameters was determined based on the Gelman and Rubin's diagnostic rule (Gelman & Rubin, 1992). Each parameter and hyperparameter was considered to have converged when the variance of the samples across the MCMC chains divided by the variance of the samples within each chain was smaller than 1.05. In the current study, all parameters of the HBM converged. 
To obtain final estimates of the CSF parameters in the HBM (peak-CS, peak-SF, and bandwidth), parameter estimates of the 6000 samples were averaged for each participant and location. The final CSFs for each participant and location were obtained by inputting the final parameter estimates to Equation 1. From these final CSFs, cutoff-SF and AULCSF were computed. These key CSF attribute values were used for statistical testing. 
P values for the average CSF key attributes (each CSF key attribute is the average of 6000 MCMC samples, as stated above) are based on permutation testing over 1000 iterations with shuffled data: the proportions of F scores or t scores in the permuted null distribution greater than or equal to the metric computed using intact data. For covariation of attributes at each location and covariation of locations for each attribute, we compared the distribution of p values of correlation coefficients (obtained from 1000 MCMC samples from the entire distribution, to match the resolution of p values to those of average CSF key attributes) against an arbitrary cutoff value of 0.05. Note that the minimum p value achievable with these procedures is 0.001, consistent with the practice to report p values up to the third decimal. All p values were false-discovery rate (FDR) corrected for multiple comparisons, when applicable (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). 
Results
We estimated CSF parameters—peak-CS (maximum sensitivity), peak-SF (the most preferred SF), and bandwidth (full width at half maximum sensitivity, related to CSF shape)—using Bayesian inference (Figure 1A). The likelihood of the observed data was computed by fitting psychometric functions with contrast thresholds (c = S–1) set as the inverse of the contrast sensitivity (S) defined by the parameters, and Bayes’ rule was used to infer the posterior distribution of the CSF parameters. Instead of fitting the CSF model separately for each participant and location, we used a HBM, which considers potential relations among parameters across the hierarchy, to accurately estimate CSF parameters (see Methods and Supplementary Figure A1). In addition, the cutoff-SF (highest perceivable SF, or SF at S = 1) and AULCSF (total window of visibility) were extracted from the fitted CSFs and are reported in the Supplementary Appendix. Higher values of these attributes indicate higher sensitivity, better performance, and/or a wider range of visible SFs and contrasts. 
The HBM were fitted to the data from the upper vertical, lower vertical, and the horizontal meridians. Data were collapsed across the left and right horizontal locations, as previous studies have shown that contrast sensitivity is similar across these two locations (e.g., Barbot et al., 2021; Cameron et al., 2002; Jigo et al., 2023). 
CSF shape is the same across polar angle locations
It is well established that peak-CS and peak-SF vary as a function of eccentricity (e.g., Virsu & Rovamo, 1979), but less is known regarding bandwidth, which determines the shape of the CSF. To test whether the CSF shape differs around polar angle, we fitted two versions of the HBM to the data (see Methods): one in which bandwidth was constrained to be the same across the three locations for each observer, and the other in which no such constraint was imposed. The Bayesian predictive information criterion (BPIC) (Ando, 2007) values used to quantify the goodness of fit was 22,141 for the fixed and 22,195 for the varying model (lower values indicate better fit). The fixed model provided statistically equivalent fits as the most saturated model, suggesting that, for each individual, the shape of the CSF does not differ around polar angle locations, and CSFs around polar angle are translationally invariant in the log(SF)–log(contrast sensitivity) space. We used the results from the fixed bandwidth model for further analyses (mean bandwidth across observers = 2.908 octaves) and report our findings focusing on peak-CS and peak-SF in the following sections (but see Figure 4). 
CSFs are enhanced at the horizontal meridian compared to the vertical meridian
To qualitatively preview the results, there were contrast sensitivity differences around polar angle at the group level (Figure 2A): Contrast sensitivity across SFs was higher at the horizontal than the vertical meridian (HVA), and higher at the lower vertical than the upper vertical meridian (VMA) (for an example participant's trial-by-trial data and the best fitting CSF models for each location, see Supplementary Figure A2). We examined each of the key CSF attributes for a detailed understanding of this pattern. 
We conducted one-way repeated-measures ANOVA, with location (upper, lower, horizontal) as a within-subject factor and observed significant location effects for peak-CS (F(2, 27) = 46.292, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.632) and peak-SF (F(2, 27) = 4.166, p = 0.025, η2 = 0.134). For peak-CS (Figure 2B), there were clear HVA and VMA. Peak-CS was significantly higher at the horizontal than the vertical meridian (average of upper and lower), indicating HVA (t(27) = 3.318, p = 0.003, d = 0.754). It was also higher at the lower vertical than the upper vertical meridian, indicating VMA (t(27) = 3.318, p < 0.001, d = 0.333). In addition, the peak-CS at the horizontal was higher than at the lower vertical meridian (t(27) = 6.001, p < 0.001, d = 0.591). 
For peak-SF (Figure 2C), we observed both HVA and VMA. The CSF peaked at a higher SF at the horizontal than at the vertical (t(27) = 2.530, p = 0.018, d = 0.102), and at the lower vertical than at the upper vertical meridian (t(27) = 2.530, p = 0.026, d = 0.387). There was no difference between the horizontal and the lower vertical meridian. The cutoff-SF and AULCSF also differed significantly as a function of polar angle, in a direction consistent with peak-CS and peak-SF (Supplementary Figure A3). These results indicate that the CSF and its corresponding attributes vary around the visual field and should not be generalized across locations. 
Individual CSFs across locations are highly correlated
Sensitivity at the group level differed around polar angle (Figure 2), indicating that the CSF and its attributes did not generalize across isoeccentric locations around the visual field. But, are individual CSFs at these locations related to each other, such that, for example, an observer with a higher peak-CS than other observers at one location would also have a higher peak-CS at another location? 
To answer this question, we analyzed subsections of the covariance matrix (Figure 1D, blue cells) and examined how each CSF attribute of an observer covaries across locations. If all CSF attributes perfectly covary across pairs of locations (Figure 1E, blue panel, top), observers’ CSFs would be shifted and scaled by the same amount between the two locations, preserving individual variability across locations. If none of the CSF attributes covary across locations (Figure 1E, blue panel, bottom), individual variability would not be preserved across locations. 
We found that the peak-CS at one location was positively correlated with their counterparts at all other locations (Figure 3A). The same pattern was observed for peak-SF (Figure 3B). Therefore, CSF attributes strongly covary across all polar angle locations (more consistent with Figure 1E, blue panel, top, than with the blue panel, bottom). These findings indicate that individual variability is preserved across all polar angle locations and suggest that an observer's CSF and its attributes at one location are good predictors of those at another location. For the respective correlations for cutoff-SF and AULCSF, see Supplementary Figure A4. For correlations from BIP outputs, see Supplementary Figure A8 (also see Methods). 
Figure 3.
 
Covariation of each CSF attribute across polar angle locations. (A) Peak contrast sensitivity. (B) Peak spatial frequency. Each cell corresponds to a blue cell in Figure 1D. The covariations of cutoff-SF and AULCSF are shown in Supplementary Figure A4, and confidence intervals are in Supplementary Figure A7. For observers’ data examples, see Supplementary Figure A5. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Figure 3.
 
Covariation of each CSF attribute across polar angle locations. (A) Peak contrast sensitivity. (B) Peak spatial frequency. Each cell corresponds to a blue cell in Figure 1D. The covariations of cutoff-SF and AULCSF are shown in Supplementary Figure A4, and confidence intervals are in Supplementary Figure A7. For observers’ data examples, see Supplementary Figure A5. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
CSFs attributes are correlated within each location and more so for the horizontal meridian
Next, we assessed the relation among CSF attributes within each location at the individual level (Figure 4). We asked: How similar or different are individual CSFs within each location, and does this extent differ around polar angle? We analyzed triangular sections below the main diagonal in the covariance matrix (Figure 1D, pink cells) to investigate whether and how key CSF attributes covary with another, across observers, separately for each location. Note that, here, the covariation of bandwidth with other CSF attributes is informative because the bandwidth was free to vary across observers (see Methods). (Note that in previous sections focusing on comparisons across locations, the bandwidth is not informative as it was fixed across locations.) The more significant correlations across pairs of CSF attributes at a location (both positive and negative), the more consistently individual CSFs at that location varied in the log(SF)–log(contrast sensitivity) space (Figure 1E, pink panel, top). The fewer the correlations among CSF attributes, the more variability among individual CSFs (Figure 1E, pink panel, bottom). 
Figure 4.
 
Covariance of CSF attributes within each location (pink cells in Figures 1D and 1E). Correlation between pairs of attributes at the (A) horizontal, (B) upper vertical, and (C) lower vertical meridian. Note that, here, the bandwidth is informative because the bandwidth varies as a function of individual observer (it is fixed only across locations). The color of each cell corresponds to the strength of correlation. The insets demonstrate the schematics of simulated individual CSFs at each location, assuming that the significant/non-significant correlations are perfect/null correlations (coefficient of 1/0) for simplicity. Each cell corresponds to a pink cell in Figure 1D. The covariation of all attributes including cutoff-SF and AULCSF are shown in Supplementary Figure A6. Confidence intervals are shown in Supplementary Figure A7. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Black and white letters are for visibility.
Figure 4.
 
Covariance of CSF attributes within each location (pink cells in Figures 1D and 1E). Correlation between pairs of attributes at the (A) horizontal, (B) upper vertical, and (C) lower vertical meridian. Note that, here, the bandwidth is informative because the bandwidth varies as a function of individual observer (it is fixed only across locations). The color of each cell corresponds to the strength of correlation. The insets demonstrate the schematics of simulated individual CSFs at each location, assuming that the significant/non-significant correlations are perfect/null correlations (coefficient of 1/0) for simplicity. Each cell corresponds to a pink cell in Figure 1D. The covariation of all attributes including cutoff-SF and AULCSF are shown in Supplementary Figure A6. Confidence intervals are shown in Supplementary Figure A7. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Black and white letters are for visibility.
The extent to which the CSF attributes correlations differed around the polar angle is depicted in Figure 4 (see Supplementary Figure A5 for examples of individual data). Only at the horizontal meridian did all CSF attributes covary with one another (Figure 4A). The peak-CS and peak-SF are positively correlated, indicating a shift across observers along the diagonal direction in the log(SF)–log(contrast sensitivity) space (Figure 4A, inset). For example, individual CSFs that are shifted more upward (peak-CS) are also shifted more rightward (peak-SF), and vice versa. In addition, bandwidth is correlated negatively with both peak-CS and peak-SF. Individual CSFs that are shifted more upward and rightward (higher peak-CS and peak-SF) are sharpened (narrower bandwidth). These significant correlations result in a more consistent organization of observers’ CSFs at the horizontal than at the other locations. 
At the upper and lower vertical meridians (Figures 4B and 4C), there was a negative correlation between bandwidth and peak-CS/peak-SF, like at the horizontal meridian. However, at these vertical locations, the peak-CS and the peak-SF were not correlated, indicating that individual CSFs do not necessarily vary along the diagonal direction. The fewer number of significant correlations results in fewer constraints on where different observers’ CSFs are located and how the attributes of their CSFs vary in the log(SF)–log(contrast sensitivity) space (Figures 4B and 4C, insets). For the covariation between cutoff-SF/AULCSF and all other attributes, see Supplementary Figure A6. For correlations from BIP outputs, see Supplementary Figure A9 and Methods
Discussion
The CSF is a building block of visual perception and an essential component of computational models of vision (Bradley, Abrams, & Geisler, 2014; Peli, 1996; Watson & Solomon, 1997). Thus, its variation with stimulus parameters has been widely assessed (DeValois & DeValois, 1990; Kelly, 1977; Van Surdam Graham, 1989; Watson, 2018)—for example, across eccentricity (Hilz & Cavonius, 1974; Jigo & Carrasco, 2020; Rovamo et al., 1978; Virsu & Rovamo, 1979). However, given the implicit or explicit assumption that CSF attributes generalize across isoeccentric locations, scarce attention has been paid to variations of the CSF around the polar angle (Jigo et al., 2023; Kwak et al., 2024). Here, we conducted a detailed investigation of the key CSF attributes at different locations not only at a group level, as in our previous studies (Jigo et al., 2023; Kwak et al., 2024), but also at an individual level using covariance analysis. This analysis revealed critical differences within and across CSF attributes around the polar angle. Across locations, we fixed the CSF shape (bandwidth) for each individual and found that the CSF and its attributes (e.g., peak-CS and peak-SF) were enhanced and shifted to higher SFs at the horizontal than at the vertical meridian and at the lower vertical than the upper vertical meridian. Moreover, each of the CSF attributes was highly correlated across polar angle locations. Within each location, the pattern in which individual observers’ CSFs differ from one another was consistent, and more so at the horizontal than the vertical meridian. 
With eccentricity, the peak-CS, peak-SF, and cutoff-SF of the CSF decrease, consistent with the drop in contrast sensitivity and spatial resolution in the periphery (Hilz & Cavonius, 1974; Jigo & Carrasco, 2020; Jigo et al., 2023; Rovamo et al., 1978; Virsu & Rovamo, 1979). As for bandwidth, results are more mixed. For example, Virsu and Rovamo (1979) reported that the attenuation in the low SF range became less pronounced at further eccentricities, indicating that the bandwidth increases from the fovea to the periphery. In contrast, Jigo et al. (2023) found no changes in bandwidth between 2° and 6°. However, it should be noted that such results could be affected by the choice of stimulus parameters (e.g., eccentricity, temporal frequency) known to affect the shape of the CSF (Baldwin et al., 2012; Cameron et al., 2002; Carrasco et al., 2001; Himmelberg et al., 2020), which differed across these studies. At the neurophysiological level, the bandwidths of CSFs of V1 cells remain constant across eccentricity (Elliott & Whitaker, 1992; Pelli, Rubin, & Legge, 1986; Thurman et al., 2016). 
Whether the bandwidth changes with eccentricity is not as clear as for other CSF attributes; thus, we compared models with fixed and varying bandwidths across polar angle locations for each individual observer. We found that CSFs at all meridians have the same shape. The most parsimonious model was the one in which the bandwidth varied across observers but was the same across locations for each observer. The shape of the CSF is related to the ratio of magnocellular cells and parvocellular cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus, which are sensitive to low and high SFs, respectively (Derrington & Lennie, 1984; Legge, 1978; Merigan & Eskin, 1986; Merigan, Katz, & Maunsell, 1991), and this ratio may be preserved at isoeccentric locations around the visual field. If a template CSF with a fixed shape can account for various conditions (e.g., polar angle), then the number of datapoints required to be sampled in stimulus space would be greatly reduced. Some studies provide evidence in support of measuring the entire CSF (Bour & Apkarian, 1996; Hess & Howell, 1977; Rohaly & Owsley, 1993), whereas other studies claim redundancy of information in these procedures (Elliott & Whitaker, 1992; Pelli et al., 1986; Thurman et al., 2016). Our findings suggest that, for each individual observer, CSFs at polar angle locations are of a fixed shape. This knowledge could greatly improve the efficiency in characterizing an observer's CSF at various locations. 
The group-level sensitivity indicated by the key attributes was best for the horizontal meridian, followed by the lower vertical, and then the upper vertical meridian, consistent with our previous studies (Figure 2) (Jigo et al., 2023; Kwak et al., 2024). Therefore, although the shape of the CSF is preserved around polar angle, CSFs need to be shifted and scaled to account for these differences. At the 6° eccentricity tested in the present study, there were clear horizontal–upper and lower–upper asymmetries. The difference between the horizontal and the lower vertical meridian was present only for the peak-CS, but differences between these two locations might emerge for other attributes when tested at farther eccentricities where these asymmetries become more pronounced (Baldwin et al., 2012; Cameron et al., 2002; Carrasco et al., 2001; Himmelberg et al., 2020). These group-level behavioral effects are related to the higher density of cone photoreceptors and retinal ganglion cells along the horizontal than the vertical meridian and of retinal ganglion cells at the lower than the upper vertical meridian (Curcio & Allen, 1990; Curcio, Sloan, Packer, Hendrickson, & Kalina, 1987; Kupers, Benson, Carrasco, & Winawer, 2022; Kupers, Carrasco, & Winawer, 2019; Song, Chui, Zhong, Elsner, & Burns, 2011; Webb & Kaas, 1976), as well as to the larger cortical surface area showing both asymmetries (Benson et al., 2021; Himmelberg et al., 2021; Himmelberg et al., 2022; Himmelberg et al., 2023). 
Importantly, we also uncovered correlations at an individual level around the visual field. First, we found that each CSF attribute is highly correlated across polar angle locations (Figure 3), indicating that an individual's CSF at one location is predictive of their CSFs at other polar angle locations. The CSFs are shifted and scaled by a similar amount for all observers across locations (e.g., a similar shifting or scaling factor across observers that explains how the CSF changes with locations); thus, the difference among individuals at a location is preserved around the visual field. At first glance, this might seem inconsistent with the group-level CSF differences around polar angle. However, it is important to note that this correlation shows that an observer with higher horizontal sensitivity (e.g., peak-CS) than other observers also has a higher vertical meridian sensitivity than other observers; this is different from the absolute values of an observer's sensitivity at these locations being similar. Taken together with the group-level CSF differences, we show for the first time, to our knowledge, that the absolute values of the CSF are different but correlated at an individual level around the visual field. 
Moreover, the way in which individual CSFs varied was more consistent at the horizontal than at the vertical meridian (Figure 4). At the horizontal locations, observers whose contrast sensitivity was higher also peaked at higher SFs, which constrained individuals’ CSFs to vary diagonally in the log(SF)–log(contrast sensitivity) space. In other words, individuals who are relatively more sensitive along the SF dimension are also more sensitive along the contrast dimension. Incorporating such relation between peak-CS and peak-SF into computational models of vision would enable a prediction of CSFs across individuals, which goes beyond predicting one observer's CSFs at multiple locations. The HBM has been applied to predicting CSFs under different luminance conditions, across observers, and for existing observers in unmeasured conditions (Lu, Zhao, Lesmes, & Dorr, 2022). A similar approach can be used for CSFs at different locations. Whether adding the constraint of the covariation between peak-CS and peak-SF into the HBM would increase its predictive power is an interesting question for future studies. However, at the vertical meridian, the covariation between peak-CS and peak-SF is less pronounced, highlighting that differences exist as a function of polar angle. Interestingly, De Valois et al. (1982) found no correlation between the peak-CS and peak-SF of V1 cell CSFs. They reported that the recording loci varied from 0° to 5° eccentricity, but it is unclear where they recorded in terms of polar angle locations. Given that, in our study, the correlation between the peak-CS and peak-SF across individuals was pronounced at the horizontal but not at the vertical meridian, it is plausible that collapsing measurements around the visual field obscured such effects in the previous study. 
At both the horizontal and the vertical meridians, peak-SF and peak-CS were negatively correlated with bandwidth (note that, although bandwidth was fixed across locations in our model, it varied with individuals). To explain these correlations, we speculate that there is a limit at the high SF range as well as on the total window of visibility (i.e., AULCSF) that the human visual system can process. If neurons can only process SFs up to a certain limit, the slope from the peak-CS to the cutoff-SF would be steeper, resulting in a narrower bandwidth for a CSF that peaks at a higher SF. Consistent with this speculation, there is neurophysiological evidence that the peak-SF and bandwidth of the CSF are negatively correlated across V1 cells (De Valois et al., 1982). Similarly, if there is a constraint on the area of the human window of visibility, a CSF peaking at a higher contrast sensitivity would be narrower. 
Some studies have investigated between-observer covariance of contrast sensitivity values across SFs to identify channels underlying the CSF (Peterzell & Teller, 1996; Peterzell, Werner, & Kaplan, 1991; Peterzell, Werner, & Kaplan, 1995). Their assumption is that contrast sensitivity values from the same channel tend to covary among observers, whereas those from different channels are generally independent. In the current study, to answer our research question, we focused on the CSF parameters that represent the entire CSF rather than obtaining independent estimates of the contrast sensitivity values at specific SFs. Future studies could use an approach that allows independently estimating contrast sensitivity values for each SF to compare how these values covary across observers for different SFs at different visual field locations. 
In conclusion, the present study unveils important relations and differences among our window of visibility around the polar angle at the group and individual levels. Key attributes of the CSF vary with location, and the pattern in which CSFs differ across observers also varies between the horizontal and the vertical meridians. However, the shape of the CSF and individual variability in CSFs are preserved across locations. Altogether, these findings call for a closer investigation of factors that are similar and different as a function of polar angle location and of the underlying neural factors. Our study highlights that comprehensive models of vision should consider CSFs around polar angles and relations among them to better depict our window of visibility around the visual field. 
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Yukai Zhao and Jonathan Winawer, as well as Rania Ezzo, Aysun Duyar, and other members of the Carrasco Lab, for helpful discussion and comments. 
Supported by a grant from the National Eye Institute, National Institutes of Health (R01-EY027401 to MC). The data will be available on https://osf.io/m8ysa upon publication, and the analysis code will be available upon reasonable request. 
Commercial relationships: Y. Kwak, None; Z.-L. Lu, Adaptive Sensory Technology, Inc. (I), Jiangsu Jeuhua Medical Technology, LTD (I); M. Carrasco, None. 
Corresponding author: Yuna Kwak. 
Email: yuna.kwak@nyu.edu. 
Address: Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA. 
References
Abrams, J., Nizam, A., & Carrasco, M. (2012). Isoeccentric locations are not equivalent: The extent of the vertical meridian asymmetry. Vision Research, 52(1), 70–78. [PubMed]
Anders, P., Traber, G. L., Hall, U., Garobbio, S. A., Chan, E. J., Gabrani, C., … Scholl, H. P. N. (2023). Evaluating contrast sensitivity in early and intermediate age-related macular degeneration with the quick contrast sensitivity function. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 64(14), 7. [PubMed]
Ando, T. (2007) Bayesian predictive information criterion for the evaluation of hierarchical Bayesian and empirical Bayes models. Biometrika, 94(2), 443–458.
Baldwin, A. S., Meese, T. S., & Baker, D. H. (2012). The attenuation surface for contrast sensitivity has the form of a witch's hat within the central visual field. Journal of Vision, 12(11):23, 1–17, https://doi.org/10.1167/12.11.23.
Baldwin, G., Vingopoulos, F., Zeng, R., Wescott, H., Bannerman, A., Koch, T., … Miller, J. B. (2023). Association between contrast sensitivity and central subfield thickness in center-involving diabetic macular edema. Journal of Vitreoretinal Diseases, 7(3), 232–238. [PubMed]
Barbot, A., Xue, S., & Carrasco, M. (2021). Asymmetries in visual acuity around the visual field. Journal of Vision, 21(1):2, 1–23, https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.1.2.
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 57(1), 289–300.
Benson, N. C., Kupers, E. R., Barbot, A., Carrasco, M., & Winawer, J. (2021). Cortical magnification in human visual cortex parallels task performance around the visual field. eLife, 10, e67685. [PubMed]
Bodis-Wollner, I. (1972). Visual acuity and contrast sensitivity in patients with cerebral lesions. Science, 178(4062), 769–771. [PubMed]
Bour, L. J., & Apkarian, P. (1996). Selective broad-band spatial frequency loss in contrast sensitivity functions. Comparison with a model based on optical transfer functions. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 37(12), 2475–2484. [PubMed]
Bradley, C., Abrams, J., & Geisler, W. S. (2014). Retina-V1 model of detectability across the visual field. Journal of Vision, 14(12):22, 1–22, https://doi.org/10.1167/14.12.22.
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436. [PubMed]
Brussee, T., van den Berg, T. J. T. P., van Nispen, R. M. A., & van Rens, G. H. M. B. (2017). Associations between spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity and reading. Optometry and Vision Science, 94(3), 329–338.
Cameron, E. L., Tai, J. C., & Carrasco, M. (2002). Covert attention affects the psychometric function of contrast sensitivity. Vision Research, 42(8), 949–967. [PubMed]
Campbell, F. W., & Robson, J. G. (1968). Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings. The Journal of Physiology, 197(3), 551–566. [PubMed]
Carrasco, M., Talgar, C. P., & Cameron, E. L. (2001). Characterizing visual performance fields: Effects of transient covert attention, spatial frequency, eccentricity, task and set size. Spatial Vision, 15(1), 61–75. [PubMed]
Chen, Z., Zhuang, Y., Xu, Z., Chan, L. Y. L., Zhang, S., Ye, Q., … Li, J. (2021). Sensitivity and stability of functional vision tests in detecting subtle changes under multiple simulated conditions. Translational Vision Science & Technology, 10(7), 7.
Chung, S., Legge, G. E., & Tjan, B.S. (2002). Spatial-frequency characteristics of letter identification in central and peripheral vision. Vision Research, 42(18), 2137–2152. [PubMed]
Cimmer, C., Szendi, I., Csifcsák, G., Szekeres, G., Ambrus Kovács, Z., Somogyi, I., … Kéri, S. (2006). Abnormal neurological signs, visual contrast sensitivity, and the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 30(7), 1225–1230. [PubMed]
Collins, J. W., & Carney, L. G. (1990). Visual performance in high myopia. Current Eye Research, 9(3), 217–223. [PubMed]
Cornelissen, F. W., Peters, E. M., & Palmer, J. (2002). The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34(4), 613–617.
Curcio, C. A., & Allen, K. A. (1990). Topography of ganglion cells in human retina. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 300(1), 5–25. [PubMed]
Curcio, C. A., Sloan, K. R., Jr., Packer, O., Hendrickson, A. E., & Kalina, R. E. (1987). Distribution of cones in human and monkey retina: Individual variability and radial asymmetry. Science, 236(4801), 579–582. [PubMed]
De Valois, R. L., Albrecht, D. G., & Thorell, L. G. (1982). Spatial frequency selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortex. Vision Research, 22(5), 545–559. [PubMed]
Derrington, A. M., & Lennie, P. (1984). Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivities of neurones in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. The Journal of Physiology, 357, 219–240. [PubMed]
DeValois, R. L., & DeValois, K. K. (1990). Spatial Vision. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Elliott, D. B., & Whitaker, D. (1992). Clinical contrast sensitivity chart evaluation. Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics, 12(3), 275–280.
Finn, M., Vingopoulos, F., Zhao, Y., Zhou, P., Bannerman, A., Romano, F., … Miller, J. B. (2024). Test-retest repeatability and agreement of the quantitative contrast sensitivity function test: Towards the validation of a new clinical endpoint. Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology, 262(3), 813–822.
Gelman, A., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Statistical Science, 7(4), 457–472.
Ginsburg, A. P. (2006). Contrast sensitivity: Determining the visual quality and function of cataract, intraocular lenses and refractive surgery. Current Opinion in Ophthalmology, 17(1), 19–26. [PubMed]
Glassman, A. R., Elmasry, M. A., Baskin, D. E., Brigell, M., Chong, V., Davis, Q., … Wenzel, A. (2024). Visual function measurements in eyes with diabetic retinopathy: An expert opinion on available measures. Ophthalmology Science, 4(5), 100519. [PubMed]
Hess, R. F., & Howell, E. R. (1977). The threshold contrast sensitivity function in strabismic amblyopia: Evidence for a two type classification. Vision Research, 17(9), 1049–1055. [PubMed]
Hilz, R., & Cavonius, C. R. (1974). Functional organization of the peripheral retina: Sensitivity to periodic stimuli. Vision Research, 14(12), 1333–1337. [PubMed]
Himmelberg, M. M., Kurzawski, J. W., Benson, N. C., Pelli, D. G., Carrasco, M., & Winawer, J. (2021). Cross-dataset reproducibility of human retinotopic maps. NeuroImage, 244, 118609. [PubMed]
Himmelberg, M. M., Tünçok, E., Gomez, J., Grill-Spector, K., Carrasco, M., & Winawer, J. (2023). Comparing retinotopic maps of children and adults reveals a late-stage change in how V1 samples the visual field. Nature Communications, 14(1), 1561. [PubMed]
Himmelberg, M. M., Winawer, J., & Carrasco, M. (2020). Stimulus-dependent contrast sensitivity asymmetries around the visual field. Journal of Vision, 20(9), 1–19.
Himmelberg, M. M., Winawer, J., & Carrasco, M. (2022). Linking individual differences in human primary visual cortex to contrast sensitivity around the visual field. Nature Communications, 13(1), 3309. [PubMed]
Hou, F., Huang, C.-B., Lesmes, L., Feng, L.-X., Tao, L., Zhou, Y.-F., ... Lu, Z.-L. (2010). qCSF in clinical application: Efficient characterization and classification of contrast sensitivity functions in amblyopia. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 51(10), 5365–5377. [PubMed]
Hou, F., Lesmes, L., Bex, P., Dorr, M., & Lu, Z.-L. (2015). Using 10AFC to further improve the efficiency of the quick CSF method. Journal of Vision, 15(9):2, 1–18, https://doi.org/10.1167/15.9.2.
Hou, F., Lu, Z.-L., & Huang, C.-B. (2014). The external noise normalized gain profile of spatial vision. Journal of Vision, 14(13):9, 1–14, https://doi.org/10.1167/14.13.9.
Jigo, M., & Carrasco, M. (2020). Differential impact of exogenous and endogenous attention on the contrast sensitivity function across eccentricity. Journal of Vision, 20(6):11, 1–25, https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.6.11.
Jigo, M., Tavdy, D., Himmelberg, M. M., & Carrasco, M. (2023). Cortical magnification eliminates differences in contrast sensitivity across but not around the visual field. eLife, 12, e84205. [PubMed]
Kelly, D. H. (1977). Visual contrast sensitivity. Optica Acta, 24(2), 107–129.
Kelly, D. H. (1979). Motion and vision. II. Stabilized spatio-temporal threshold surface. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 69(10), 1340–1349. [PubMed]
Kupers, E. R., Benson, N. C., Carrasco, M., & Winawer, J. (2022). Asymmetries around the visual field: From retina to cortex to behavior. PLoS Computational Biology, 18(1), e1009771. [PubMed]
Kupers, E. R., Carrasco, M., & Winawer, J. (2019). Modeling visual performance differences “around” the visual field: A computational observer approach. PLoS Computational Biology, 15(5), e1007063. [PubMed]
Kwak, Y., Zhao, Y., Lu, Z.-L., Hanning, N. M., & Carrasco, M. (2024). Presaccadic attention enhances and reshapes the Contrast Sensitivity Function differentially around the visual field. eNeuro, 11(9), ENEURO.0243–24.2024. [PubMed]
Legge, G. E. (1978). Sustained and transient mechanisms in human vision: Temporal and spatial properties. Vision Research, 18(1), 69–81. [PubMed]
Lesmes, L. A., Lu, Z.-L., Baek, J., & Albright, T. D. (2010). Bayesian adaptive estimation of the contrast sensitivity function: The quick CSF method. Journal of Vision, 10(3):17, 1–21, https://doi.org/10.1167/10.3.17.
Levi, D. M., & Li, R. W. (2009). Improving the performance of the amblyopic visual system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1515), 399–407. [PubMed]
Lu, Z.-L., Zhao, Y., Lesmes, L. A., & Dorr, M. (2022). Predicting contrast sensitivity functions with digital twins generated by hierarchical Bayesian modelling. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 63(7), 2328.
Majaj, N. J., Pelli, D. G., Kurshan, P., & Palomares, M. (2002). The role of spatial frequency channels in letter identification. Vision Research, 42(9), 1165–1184. [PubMed]
Marron, J. A., & Bailey, I. L. (1982). Visual factors and orientation-mobility performance. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics, 59(5), 413–426. [PubMed]
McAnany, J. J., & Levine, M. W. (2007). Magnocellular and parvocellular visual pathway contributions to visual field anisotropies. Vision Research, 47(17), 2327–2336. [PubMed]
Merigan, W. H., & Eskin, T. A. (1986). Spatio-temporal vision of macaques with severe loss of Pβ retinal ganglion cells. Vision Research, 26(11), 1751–1761. [PubMed]
Merigan, W. H., Katz, L. M., & Maunsell, J. H. (1991). The effects of parvocellular lateral geniculate lesions on the acuity and contrast sensitivity of macaque monkeys. The Journal of Neuroscience, 11(4), 994–1001.
Owsley, C. (2003). Contrast sensitivity. Ophthalmology Clinics of North America, 16(2), 171–177. [PubMed]
Owsley, C., & McGwin, G., Jr. (2010). Vision and driving. Vision Research, 50(23), 2348–2361. [PubMed]
Peli, E. (1996). Test of a model of foveal vision by using simulations. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 13(6), 1131–1138. [PubMed]
Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437–442. [PubMed]
Pelli, D. G., & Bex, P. (2013). Measuring contrast sensitivity. Vision Research, 90, 10–14. [PubMed]
Pelli, D. G., Rubin, G. S., & Legge, G. E. (1986). Predicting the contrast sensitivity of low vision observers. In Annual Meeting Optical Society of America. Washington, DC: Optica Publishing Group.
Peterzell, D. H., & Teller, D. Y. (1996). Individual differences in contrast sensitivity functions: The lowest spatial frequency channels. Vision Research, 36(19), 3077–3085. [PubMed]
Peterzell, D. H., Werner, J. S., & Kaplan, P. S. (1991). Individual differences in contrast sensitivity functions of human adults and infants: A brief review. In Bagnoli, P. & Hodos, W. (Eds.), The changing visual system: Maturation and aging in the central nervous system (pp. 391–396). New York: Springer Science+Business Media.
Peterzell, D. H., Werner, J. S., & Kaplan, P. S. (1995). Individual differences in contrast sensitivity functions: Longitudinal study of 4-, 6- and 8-month-old human infants. Vision Research, 35(7), 961–979. [PubMed]
Robson, J. G. (1966). Spatial and temporal contrast-sensitivity functions of the visual system. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 56(8), 1141.
Rohaly, A. M., & Owsley, C. (1993). Modeling the contrast-sensitivity functions of older adults. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics and Image Science, 10(7), 1591–1599. [PubMed]
Rovamo, J., Luntinen, O., & Näsänen, R. (1993). Modelling the dependence of contrast sensitivity on grating area and spatial frequency. Vision Research, 33(18), 2773–2788. [PubMed]
Rovamo, J., & Virsu, V. (1979). An estimation and application of the human cortical magnification factor. Experimental Brain Research, 37(3), 495–510. [PubMed]
Rovamo, J., Virsu, V., & Näsänen, R. (1978). Cortical magnification factor predicts the photopic contrast sensitivity of peripheral vision. Nature, 271(5640), 54–56. [PubMed]
Rubin, G. S., & Legge, G. E. (1989). Psychophysics of reading. VI—The role of contrast in low vision. Vision Research, 29(1), 79–91. [PubMed]
Schieber, F., Kline, D. W., Kline, T. J. B., & Fozard, J. L. (1992). The relationship between contrast sensitivity and the visual problems of older drivers. Warrendale, PA: SAE International.
Song, H., Chui, T. Y. P., Zhong, Z., Elsner, A. E., & Burns, S. A. (2011). Variation of cone photoreceptor packing density with retinal eccentricity and age. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 52(10), 7376–7384. [PubMed]
Thurman, S. M., Davey, P. G., McCray, K. L., Paronian, V., & Seitz, A. R. (2016). Predicting individual contrast sensitivity functions from acuity and letter contrast sensitivity measurements. Journal of Vision, 16(15):15, 1–15, https://doi.org/10.1167/16.15.15. [PubMed]
Van Surdam Graham, N. (1989). Visual pattern analyzers. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Virsu, V., & Rovamo, J. (1979). Visual resolution, contrast sensitivity, and the cortical magnification factor. Experimental Brain Research, 37(3), 475–494. [PubMed]
Watson, A. B. (2013). High frame rates and human vision: A view through the window of visibility. SMPTE Motion Imaging Journal, 122(2), 18–32.
Watson, A. B. (2018). The field of view, the field of resolution, and the field of contrast sensitivity. Journal of Perceptual Imaging, 1(1), 010505-1–010505-11.
Watson, A. B., & Ahumada, A. J., Jr. (2005). A standard model for foveal detection of spatial contrast. Journal of Vision, 5(9):6, 717–740, https://doi.org/10.1167/5.9.6. [PubMed]
Watson, A. B., Ahumada, A. J., & Farrell, J. E. (1986). Window of visibility: A psychophysical theory of fidelity in time-sampled visual motion displays. The Journal of the Optical Society of America, 3(3), 300–307.
Watson, A. B., & Solomon, J. A. (1997). Model of visual contrast gain control and pattern masking. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 14(9), 2379–2391. [PubMed]
Webb, S. V., & Kaas, J. H. (1976). The sizes and distribution of ganglion cells in the retina of the owl monkey, Aotus trivirgatus. Vision Research, 16(11), 1247–1254. [PubMed]
Wichmann, F. A., & Hill, N. J. (2001). The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Perception & Psychophysics, 63(8), 1293–1313. [PubMed]
Wolffsohn, J. S., Eperjesi, F., & Napper, G. (2005). Evaluation of Melbourne Edge Test contrast sensitivity measures in the visually impaired. Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics, 25(4), 371–374.
Xu, Z., Zhuang, Y., Chen, Z., Hou, F., Chan, L. Y. L., Feng, L., … Li, J. (2022). Assessing the contrast sensitivity function in myopic parafovea: A quick contrast sensitivity functions study. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 16, 971009. [PubMed]
Zhao, Y., Lesmes, L. A., Dorr, M., & Lu, Z.-L. (2021). Quantifying uncertainty of the estimated visual acuity behavioral function with hierarchical Bayesian modeling. Translational Vision Science & Technology, 10(12), 18. [PubMed]
Zhao, Y., Lesmes, L. A., Hou, F., & Lu, Z.-L. (2021). Hierarchical Bayesian modeling of contrast sensitivity functions in a within-subject design. Journal of Vision, 21(12):9, 1–17, https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.12.9.
Zhou, Y., Huang, C., Xu, P., Tao, L., Qiu, Z., Li, X., & Lu, Z.-L. (2006). Perceptual learning improves contrast sensitivity and visual acuity in adults with anisometropic amblyopia. Vision Research, 46(5), 739–750. [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
Experimental schematics and procedure. (A) CSF analysis. The CSF depicts how contrast sensitivity depends on SF. The inverse of the contrast threshold (c–1) is contrast sensitivity (S) at a particular SF. Bayesian inference was used to estimate the CSF parameters: peak-CS (maximum sensitivity), peak-SF (the most preferred SF), and bandwidth (closely related to CSF shape). Bandwidth was fixed across locations (see Methods). Results of the cutoff-SF (highest discernable SF) and AULCSF (total sensitivity) are reported in Supplementary Appendix. Higher values of these attributes are related to higher sensitivity, better performance, and/or a wider range of visible SFs and contrasts. (B) Polar angle asymmetries. In many visual dimensions, performance is higher at the horizontal than the vertical meridian (HVA) and at the lower than the upper vertical meridian (VMA). (C) Task. A test Gabor stimulus was presented briefly at one of the four polar angle locations, and participants judged its orientation (clockwise or counterclockwise). SF and contrast of the stimulus varied on each trial. (D) Schematics of covariance analysis. The covariance of the three CSF attributes (peak-CS, peak-SF, bandwidth) for all three locations is a 9 × 9 matrix. The covariation of locations for each attribute (blue cells) and the covariation of attributes within each location (pink cells) were assessed. Covariance for bandwidth is shaded in gray because the bandwidth was identical across locations for each observer in the HBM and, thus, the covariance was not informative in most cases (but see Figure 4). (E) Interpretation of covariance analysis. Covariance of locations for each attribute (D, blue cells) tests whether CSFs and their corresponding key attributes covary as a function of polar angle. For example, a perfect positive correlation (coefficient of 1) for peak-CS among locations X and Y indicates that an observer with a higher peak-CS at location X than other individuals also has a higher peak-CS at location Y (E, blue panel, top). This relation does not hold for a scenario where there is no correlation (coefficient of 0) for any CSF attributes between locations X and Y. Covariance within each location (D, pink cells) allows investigation of how individuals’ CSFs relate to one another within each location. A perfect positive covariation (correlation coefficient of 1) of all combinations of attributes within a location indicates that observers’ CSFs are organized diagonally in the SF-contrast space: The higher the peak-CS, the higher the peak-SF and the wider the bandwidth (E, pink panel, top). The fewer the correlations, the greater the variability in the pattern of individual differences (E, pink panel, bottom, is a scenario where no correlations are present among CSF attributes within a location).
Figure 1.
 
Experimental schematics and procedure. (A) CSF analysis. The CSF depicts how contrast sensitivity depends on SF. The inverse of the contrast threshold (c–1) is contrast sensitivity (S) at a particular SF. Bayesian inference was used to estimate the CSF parameters: peak-CS (maximum sensitivity), peak-SF (the most preferred SF), and bandwidth (closely related to CSF shape). Bandwidth was fixed across locations (see Methods). Results of the cutoff-SF (highest discernable SF) and AULCSF (total sensitivity) are reported in Supplementary Appendix. Higher values of these attributes are related to higher sensitivity, better performance, and/or a wider range of visible SFs and contrasts. (B) Polar angle asymmetries. In many visual dimensions, performance is higher at the horizontal than the vertical meridian (HVA) and at the lower than the upper vertical meridian (VMA). (C) Task. A test Gabor stimulus was presented briefly at one of the four polar angle locations, and participants judged its orientation (clockwise or counterclockwise). SF and contrast of the stimulus varied on each trial. (D) Schematics of covariance analysis. The covariance of the three CSF attributes (peak-CS, peak-SF, bandwidth) for all three locations is a 9 × 9 matrix. The covariation of locations for each attribute (blue cells) and the covariation of attributes within each location (pink cells) were assessed. Covariance for bandwidth is shaded in gray because the bandwidth was identical across locations for each observer in the HBM and, thus, the covariance was not informative in most cases (but see Figure 4). (E) Interpretation of covariance analysis. Covariance of locations for each attribute (D, blue cells) tests whether CSFs and their corresponding key attributes covary as a function of polar angle. For example, a perfect positive correlation (coefficient of 1) for peak-CS among locations X and Y indicates that an observer with a higher peak-CS at location X than other individuals also has a higher peak-CS at location Y (E, blue panel, top). This relation does not hold for a scenario where there is no correlation (coefficient of 0) for any CSF attributes between locations X and Y. Covariance within each location (D, pink cells) allows investigation of how individuals’ CSFs relate to one another within each location. A perfect positive covariation (correlation coefficient of 1) of all combinations of attributes within a location indicates that observers’ CSFs are organized diagonally in the SF-contrast space: The higher the peak-CS, the higher the peak-SF and the wider the bandwidth (E, pink panel, top). The fewer the correlations, the greater the variability in the pattern of individual differences (E, pink panel, bottom, is a scenario where no correlations are present among CSF attributes within a location).
Figure 2.
 
Polar angle sensitivity differences. (A) CSFs. An example participant's data are shown in Supplementary Figure A2. (B) Peak contrast sensitivity. (C) Peak spatial frequency. Results for cutoff-SF and AULCSF are shown in Supplementary Figure A3. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Error bars are ±1 SEM. HVA and VMA denote horizontal-vertical anisotropy and vertical meridian asymmetries, respectively.
Figure 2.
 
Polar angle sensitivity differences. (A) CSFs. An example participant's data are shown in Supplementary Figure A2. (B) Peak contrast sensitivity. (C) Peak spatial frequency. Results for cutoff-SF and AULCSF are shown in Supplementary Figure A3. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Error bars are ±1 SEM. HVA and VMA denote horizontal-vertical anisotropy and vertical meridian asymmetries, respectively.
Figure 3.
 
Covariation of each CSF attribute across polar angle locations. (A) Peak contrast sensitivity. (B) Peak spatial frequency. Each cell corresponds to a blue cell in Figure 1D. The covariations of cutoff-SF and AULCSF are shown in Supplementary Figure A4, and confidence intervals are in Supplementary Figure A7. For observers’ data examples, see Supplementary Figure A5. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Figure 3.
 
Covariation of each CSF attribute across polar angle locations. (A) Peak contrast sensitivity. (B) Peak spatial frequency. Each cell corresponds to a blue cell in Figure 1D. The covariations of cutoff-SF and AULCSF are shown in Supplementary Figure A4, and confidence intervals are in Supplementary Figure A7. For observers’ data examples, see Supplementary Figure A5. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Figure 4.
 
Covariance of CSF attributes within each location (pink cells in Figures 1D and 1E). Correlation between pairs of attributes at the (A) horizontal, (B) upper vertical, and (C) lower vertical meridian. Note that, here, the bandwidth is informative because the bandwidth varies as a function of individual observer (it is fixed only across locations). The color of each cell corresponds to the strength of correlation. The insets demonstrate the schematics of simulated individual CSFs at each location, assuming that the significant/non-significant correlations are perfect/null correlations (coefficient of 1/0) for simplicity. Each cell corresponds to a pink cell in Figure 1D. The covariation of all attributes including cutoff-SF and AULCSF are shown in Supplementary Figure A6. Confidence intervals are shown in Supplementary Figure A7. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Black and white letters are for visibility.
Figure 4.
 
Covariance of CSF attributes within each location (pink cells in Figures 1D and 1E). Correlation between pairs of attributes at the (A) horizontal, (B) upper vertical, and (C) lower vertical meridian. Note that, here, the bandwidth is informative because the bandwidth varies as a function of individual observer (it is fixed only across locations). The color of each cell corresponds to the strength of correlation. The insets demonstrate the schematics of simulated individual CSFs at each location, assuming that the significant/non-significant correlations are perfect/null correlations (coefficient of 1/0) for simplicity. Each cell corresponds to a pink cell in Figure 1D. The covariation of all attributes including cutoff-SF and AULCSF are shown in Supplementary Figure A6. Confidence intervals are shown in Supplementary Figure A7. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Black and white letters are for visibility.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×