Outliers were excluded from the datasets before analyses were carried out. In the present experiment, the mean response times of four subjects were regarded as outliers because they did not complete all experimental tasks carefully according to the instruction. This study examined the ability to recognize a figure-eight shape under single-task conditions for the DD and CA groups using a single task as the baseline condition. The mean accuracy rate of SI identification under the single-task condition was computed using an independent-sample
t-test. The results showed that the differences were statistically significant for the DD and CA groups; the accuracy rate of the DD group was significantly lower than that of the CA group,
t(16) = −2.27,
p < 0.05, Cohen's
d = 1.02. This was due to differences in the mean accuracy rates of SI identification between the DD and CA groups under the single-task condition. Based on prior research (
Dispaldro et al., 2013), to obtain a general index of the attentional engagement deficit and to control for any effects attributed to perceptual masking, the S2I (S2I refers to the masked effect was removed by subtracting the AM effect from the accuracy scores in the single-task condition) was calculated under the dual-task condition.
Table 1 presents the group differences in the single- and dual-task conditions.
We adopted a repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the mean accuracy rate of S2I with SOAs (140 and 250 ms vs. 600 ms) and spatial position (left, middle, or right) as within-subject variables and group (DD vs. CA) as a between-subject variable. The results showed that the SOA main effect was significant, F(1, 32) = 5.05, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.24; that is, the mean accuracy rate for 140 ms was significantly lower than that for 250 ms and 600 ms. The main effect of spatial position was significant, F(1, 32) = 7.94, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.33, and the mean accuracy rate in the middle position was significantly smaller than in the left position, but there was no significant difference between the middle and right positions. In addition, the group main effect was not significant, F(1, 16) = 0.55, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.03. More importantly, the interaction between SOA and spatial position was significant, F(4, 64) = 2.87, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.15, indicating a significant difference in spatial position for the SOA of 140 ms, F(2, 34) = 7.32, p < 0.05, but there was no significant difference in spatial position for the SOAs of 250 ms or 600 ms (p > 0.05). It is noteworthy that the interaction among SOA, spatial position, and group was significant, F(4, 64) = 2.57, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.14.
For DD, there was a significant difference in the mean accuracy rate of the spatial position variable only for the SOA of 140 ms,
F(2, 18) = 6.70,
p < 0.05. Further analysis indicated that the mean accuracy rate of DD in the middle position was significantly lower than that in the left position, but there was no significant difference compared to the right position.
Figure 2A presents the results. For CA, there was no significant difference in the mean accuracy rate of the spatial position variables for the SOAs of 140, 250, and 600 ms (
p > 0.05) (
Figure 2B). The research results indicate differences in the visual attention masking effects between DD and CA at different SOAs and spatial positions.
To comprehensively analyze the effects of the three different spatial positions on the visual attention engagement of the CA and DD, we adopted a repeated-measures ANOVA with SOA (140 and 250 ms vs. 600 ms) as the within-subject variable and group (DD and CA) as the between-subject variable. In the left position, the main effect of SOA was not significant, F(2, 32) = 0.29, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.018, nor was the main effect of group, F(1, 16) = 1.67, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.095. Moreover, the interaction between SOA and group was not significant, F(2, 32) = 0.58, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.035. Meanwhile, in the right position, the main effect of SOA and group was not significant, F(2, 32) = 1.64, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.093; F(1, 16) = 0.189, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.012, and the interaction between SOA and group was not significant, F(2, 32) = 2.778, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.148.
More importantly, in the middle position, there was a significant main effect of SOA,
F(2, 32) =10.63,
p < 0.05, η
2 = 0.399, reflecting larger AM effects for the 140-ms condition than the 250-ms and 600-ms conditions. The main effect of group was not significant,
F(1,16) = 0.096,
p > 0.05, η
2 = 0.006, but the interaction between SOA and group was significant,
F(2, 32) = 4.68,
p < 0.05, η
2 = 0.227, reflecting that the AM effect for the three different SOAs varied across groups. Specifically, for the DD group, SOA showed a significant effect on the AM effect,
F(2, 32) = 16.37,
p < 0.05, indicating that the AM effect was significantly larger for the 140-ms SOA than the 250-ms and 600-ms SOAs. In contrast, for the CA group, SOA had no significant effect on the AM effect,
F(2, 32) = 0.69,
p > 0.05 (
Figure 3).