Figure 8 shows the
c values for the target and distractor manipulations. Another version of these plots, with different colors for each participant, can be found in the
Supplementary Materials. The
c values were negative for both manipulations, indicating an overall incongruency bias. When calculating
c based on the rate of correct –45° and incorrect +45° responses, it was close to zero. When calculating it splitting the data based on distractor orientation, the incongruency bias was confirmed. These results are reported in the
Supplementary Materials.
Figure 9 shows the
d’ values for each manipulation separately for the congruent and incongruent groups. Another version of these plots, with different colors for each participant, can be found in the
Supplementary Materials. The average
d’ for the congruent condition was 0.764, and it was 1.883 for the incongruent condition. For the congruent condition, the mixed-model ANOVAs on the three manipulations confirmed significant effects: target eccentricity,
F(1, 35) = 27.893,
p < 0.01; distractors,
F(1, 35) = 17.483,
p < 0.01; and gap position,
F(1, 35) = 71.774,
p < 0.01. We also found significant effects for all the manipulations in the incongruent condition: target eccentricity,
F(1, 35) = 4.670,
p = 0.37; distractors,
F(1, 35) = 7.168,
p = 0.11; and gap position,
F(1, 35) = 16.394,
p < 0.01.
Similar results were found when the data were divided for the orientation of the central lines. For the –45° group, all three models revealed a significant effect of the variable taken into account: target eccentricity manipulation, F(1, 35) = 22.887, p < 0.01; distractor manipulation, F(1, 35) = 6.644, p = 0.14; and gap position, F(1, 35) = 51.264, p < 0.01. The same results occurred for the +45° group: target eccentricity manipulation, F(1, 35) = 11.291, p = 0.02; distractor manipulation, F(1, 35) = 15.323, p < 0.01; and gap position, F(1, 35) = 67.631, p < 0.01.