Open Access
Article  |   April 2025
Binding in visual working memory is task dependent
Author Affiliations
Journal of Vision April 2025, Vol.25, 4. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.25.4.4
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Ruoyi Cao, Leon Y. Deouell; Binding in visual working memory is task dependent. Journal of Vision 2025;25(4):4. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.25.4.4.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Working memory is a neurocognitive system for maintaining and manipulating information online for a short period after the source of information disappears. The information held in working memory has been shown to flexibly match current functional goals. Considering this, we revisited the question of whether information is held in working memory as separate features or as bound objects. We conjectured that, rather than having a fixed answer, the format in which information is maintained in working memory is also task dependent. In two separate experiments, we investigated the binding between features when the location was not (Experiment 1, color and orientation binding) or was (Experiment 2, color and location binding) a task-relevant feature in a delayed (yes/no) recognition task by manipulating the relative relevance of conjunctions and separate features. Each experiment included two conditions: binding dominant (BD), which emphasized the retention of binding between features, and feature dominant (FD), which emphasized the retention of individual features. In both experiments, we found that memory for conjunctions was better in the BD condition and that memory for separate features was better in the FD condition. These patterns suggested that the formats of objects in visual working memory could be shaped by the tasks they serve. Additionally, we found that the memory of location was impaired when the conjunction between location and color was task irrelevant, whereas the memory of color and the memory of orientation were relatively independent of each other. We conclude that the representation format of objects in working memory is influenced by task requirements.

Introduction
Visual working memory (VWM) refers to the function of preserving and manipulating visual information for a short duration when physical stimuli are absent from direct perception (Baddeley, 1992; D'Esposito & Postle, 2015; Goldman-Rakic, 1995). Studies in both animals (Gilad, Gallero-Salas, Groos, & Helmchen, 2018; Rao, Rainer, & Miller, 1997) and humans (Myers, Stokes, & Nobre, 2017) regard VWM as a dynamic system that prioritizes task-relevant information rather than as fixed short-term storage. This was demonstrated, for example, by studies showing that VWM can selectively maintain task-relevant locations (Griffin & Nobre, 2003) or other features according to task demand (Park, Sy, Hong, & Tong, 2017; Pertzov, Bays, Joseph, & Husain, 2013; Ye, Hu, Ristaniemi, Gendron, & Liu, 2016). In this study, we investigated the flexibility of another level—namely, whether separate features or bound objects are the units of storage, which we refer to below as the storage format. 
Studies investigating the storage format of objects in VWM that rely on the notion of limited VWM capacity, have yielded conflicting results. Specifically, some studies have suggested that the number of objects (conjoined features), rather than the number of features, is the limiting factor (Luria & Vogel, 2011; Woodman, Vogel, & Luck, 2001), whereas others have found that the number of features limits performance (An, Gong, McLoughlin, Yang, & Wang, 2014; Delvenne & Bruyer, 2004; Fougnie, Asplund, & Marois, 2010; Oberauer & Eichenberger, 2013; Olson & Jiang, 2002). One of the explored factors is whether the relevant features come from the same dimension. According to the multiple-resources view proposed by Wheeler and Treisman (2002), VWM maintains features from different feature dimensions (e.g., color and orientation) in parallel, with no additional cost, whereas features from the same feature dimension (e.g., two colors) compete for storage space (Wang, 2017; Wheeler & Treisman, 2002). In this study, we focused on binding between features from different dimensions. 
There is contradictory evidence regarding the retention of orientation and color in VWM. Some studies have found similar memory performance for orientation and color conjunctions as for the memory of only one dimension, suggesting conjoined maintenance (Luck & Vogel, 1997). Others have found that, when participants were asked to recall both the color and orientation of an object, errors in memory for one feature did not significantly affect memory for the other feature, suggesting separate storage (Fougnie & Alvarez, 2011). However, although the quantity of recalled objects was largely unaffected by increasing the number of features, the precision of these representations dramatically decreased (Fougnie et al., 2010). 
Spatial location information may have a special status in regard to object processing. Theoretical considerations suggest that space is extrinsic to the object; thus, object-to-location binding is believed to mediate the binding of other features (Fougnie & Marois, 2009; Schneegans & Bays, 2017). According to feature integration theory, spatial attention is thought to be serially directed to a “master map.” At any given moment, one of the locations on the map is selected by the “window of attention.” Features located within this window are then assembled in a single object file for further analysis and identification (Treisman, 1977). Like binding between two non-location features, mixed results were obtained when binding with location was involved. Some studies have suggested that objects are automatically bound to their locations. This claim is supported by the above-chance decoding accuracy of task-irrelevant location (Foster, Bsales, Jaffe, & Awh, 2017), by impaired recognition performance when memoranda appear at a location different from where they were encoded (Hollingworth, 2007; Olson & Marshuetz, 2005), and by the activation of retinotopic organization in visual regions during memory delay (Dicarlo & Maunsell, 2003; Op De Beeck & Vogels, 2000). However, other studies have provided evidence for separate representations. Lesion studies (Darling, Della Sala, Logie, & Cantagallo, 2006), multiple object tracking tasks (Kondo & Saiki, 2012), and behavioral interference studies (Darling, Della Sala, & Logie, 2007) have shown a dissociation in performance between location and identity memory. Studies have also shown that memory for bound objects could be impaired without impairment in memory for individual features or locations in normal participants (Pertzov, Dong, Peich, & Husain, 2012), patients with a variant of encephalitis (Pertzov, Miller, et al., 2013) and patients with Alzheimer's disease (Liang et al., 2016). 
There are a few hints in the literature to suggest that the heterogeneity of tasks might at least partially explain some of the different results, as different tasks make conjunctions more or less relevant. Regarding the conjunction of location with objects, one study (Ester, Serences, & Awh, 2009) found that the decoding of items from blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) signals was similar between the two hemispheres, ipsilateral and contralateral to the object held in memory, suggesting that visual details were held in VWM through spatially global recruitment of the early sensory cortex. However, by making the location task relevant, Pratte and Tong (2014) found that decoding accuracy for remembered items was greater on the contralateral side than on the ipsilateral side. 
Thus, we conjectured that the storage format (separate features or conjunctions) is flexible, and it is the task that determines how information is maintained in VWM. We focused on features from separate dimensions and investigated the effect of task requirements on feature binding with (color–location) and without (color–orientation) the involvement of spatial location. In contrast to studies that manipulated the relevance of locations or other features, we were interested in directly manipulating the relevance of conjunctions. 
We conducted two experiments to investigate the adaptivity of maintenance formats in WM for binding between features with and without the location as a task-relevant dimension. We surmised that, even without explicitly informing subjects about the preferred mode of maintenance, the format of items in working memory would be flexibly adjusted to optimally perform the task. In a delayed (yes/no) recognition task, participants were presented with two items to be remembered. Subsequently, they had to indicate whether a probe, presented after a delay, had been shown in the initial array. Two types of probes were used: binding probes and feature probes. Binding probes required the memory of conjunction between features, whereas the feature probes did not. The experiments contained two separate blocks with different proportions of binding probes and feature probes: binding dominant (BD) and feature dominant (FD). Consequently, conjunction memory was more task relevant in the BD condition than in the FD condition. We predicted an interaction between condition (BD and FD) and probe type (binding probe and feature probe), such that performance for binding probes would be better in the BD condition, and performance for feature probes would be better in the FD condition. 
Experiment 1
The aim of this experiment was to test whether task requirements could influence working memory retention of conjunctions of two non-location features, color and orientation. We tacitly manipulated the optimal strategy by presenting more frequently either probes requiring conjunction information (binding probes) or probes requiring only feature information (feature probes). We expected to find that, when conjunctions were tested more frequently, items in working memory would be maintained as bound objects, improving conjunction recognition, whereas when separate features were tested more frequently items in working memory would be maintained as separate features, impairing conjunction recognition. 
Materials and methods
Participants
Thirty-one students were recruited through the Hebrew University online study recruitment system (age range, 21–36 years; mean age ± SD, 24.79 ± 2.45 years; 80% females). Twenty-four of them passed the inclusion criteria described later. All participants reported no neurological ailments. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Social Sciences Faculty at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel. Participants downloaded the computer program made with the E-prime 3 online version (https://pstnet.com/eprime-go/) and performed the experiment on their own computers. Each participant conducted the two conditions of the experiment, each on different dates within a week. They received either two course credits or a monetary compensation of 60 NISs (∼$15) for their participation. 
Stimuli
The items were Gabor gratings (five cycles, contrast 0.7) (Figure 1). The exact visual angle of the stimulus varied as participants performed the tests on their own screens. Each memory array was comprised of two Gabor gratings, with each grating having a diameter of 300 pixels. One grating was positioned on the right side of the screen, and the other was located on the left side. The distance between the center of each Gabor grating and the screen center was 200 pixels. Each grating was assigned a color from a set of four distinct colors—red (255, 0, 0), green (0, 255, 0), magenta (255, 0, 255), and blue (0, 0, 255)—with an orientation out of four possible angles (3°, 49°, 95°, or 141° relative to the horizontal line). No two items within the same array had the same color or orientation, resulting in a total of 144 unique combinations. A subset of 108 of these combinations was randomly selected without repetition and used consistently across all subjects. The stimuli were created using Psychtoolbox-3, implemented in MATLAB 2018 (MathWorks, Natick, MA). Unlike the memory array, each probe contained only one grating. There were two types of probes (Figure 1b): binding and feature. 
Figure 1.
 
Design of Experiment 1. (a, left panel) Example of a binding trial with a neither-match probe in the main blocks. (a, right panel) Example of a feature trial with a color-match probe. (b) Examples of five match states of binding probes and four match states of feature probes labeled in relation to the array shown in panel a. The numbers represent the number of trials for each match state included in the main block of BD and FD conditions, respectively. (c) Experimental procedure. Both conditions began with two divided practice blocks, one consisting of trials with binding probes and the other consisting of trials with feature probes. This was followed by a combined practice block, where both types of probes were presented in a random order. Each participant then proceeded to complete two cycles, starting with an induction block followed by two main blocks. The induction blocks consisted of only binding trials in the BD condition and only feature probes in the FD condition. In the main block, the majority of probes in the BD condition (72 out of 108) were binding trials, whereas the majority of probes in the FD condition (72 out of 108) were feature trials.
Figure 1.
 
Design of Experiment 1. (a, left panel) Example of a binding trial with a neither-match probe in the main blocks. (a, right panel) Example of a feature trial with a color-match probe. (b) Examples of five match states of binding probes and four match states of feature probes labeled in relation to the array shown in panel a. The numbers represent the number of trials for each match state included in the main block of BD and FD conditions, respectively. (c) Experimental procedure. Both conditions began with two divided practice blocks, one consisting of trials with binding probes and the other consisting of trials with feature probes. This was followed by a combined practice block, where both types of probes were presented in a random order. Each participant then proceeded to complete two cycles, starting with an induction block followed by two main blocks. The induction blocks consisted of only binding trials in the BD condition and only feature probes in the FD condition. In the main block, the majority of probes in the BD condition (72 out of 108) were binding trials, whereas the majority of probes in the FD condition (72 out of 108) were feature trials.
Binding probes
Each binding probe was classified into one of five match states: matched, mis-conjunct, new-color, new-orientation, or neither-match (Figure 1b). A matched probe shared both color and orientation with one of the items in the memory array. A mis-conjunct probe had the color of one of the items in the memory array (“old” color) with the orientation of the other item (“old” orientation). In a new-color probe, the orientation of one of the items in the memory array was paired with a “new” color that was not present in the array, and vice versa for the new-orientation probe. Finally, in the neither-match probe, neither the color nor orientation of the probe was present in the memory array. 
Feature probes
Feature probes contained only one of the two relevant dimensions: color or orientation (Figure 1a). A color probe consisted of an array of small, filled circles within a square 300 pixels in length in either old (color-match probe) or new (new-color probe) colors, presented at the center of the screen. An orientation probe consisted of a colorless gray Gabor grating presented with an orientation either present in the memory array (orientation-match probe) or not (new-orientation probe). 
Thus, memory arrays were similar in binding and feature trials, and the appearance of the probe informed the subjects whether they needed to perform color recognition, orientation recognition, or color-orientation conjunction recognition. 
Procedure
The two conditions, BD and FD, were administered to the same subject on separate days (within-subject design). In the BD condition, the majority of trials presented binding probes; in the FD condition, the majority of trials presented feature probes. The order of conditions was counterbalanced between subjects, and the order of trials was randomized within each subject. Each condition consisted of four block types: two divided practice blocks, one combined practice block, one induction block, and two main blocks. The combination of one induction block with two main blocks was repeated twice (Figure 1c). 
Divided practice blocks
The experiment began with two practice blocks with 36 trials each. One block included only trials with binding probes, and the other consisted of only trials with feature probes. The order of these two practice blocks was counterbalanced across subjects to minimize potential order effects. Each practice trial began with a 100-ms fixation cross presented at the center of the screen. Next, the memory array appeared for 400 ms, followed by a 1-second blank screen. After the delay period, a probe item was presented, and participants had to press the “J” key on their keyboard to indicate that the probe item was a “match” (i.e., present in the memory array) or the “F” key to indicate that the probe item was a “non-match.” The probe disappeared when the response was made or 3 seconds elapsed. Feedback was then given for 300 ms, indicating whether the response was correct, incorrect, or too late (>3 seconds). 
Combined practice block
Following the divided practice blocks, participants proceeded to a combined practice block. The trial procedure in this block was the same as that in the divided practice blocks. However, this block presented both types of probes: Four binding probe trials and four feature probe trials, in random order. By exposing participants to a mix of probe types, the combined practice block familiarized subjects with switching between different types of probes. 
Induction block
After the practice phase, participants engaged in an induction block with 24 trials. In the FD condition, the induction block consisted of only feature probes, with six trials for each probe type (color-match, orientation-match, new-color, new-orientation). In the BD session, the induction block consisted of only binding probes (12 matched probes, six mis-conjunct probes, and two each of new-color, new-orientation, and neither-match probes). The aim of this block was to induce a specific maintenance format before the main testing blocks began. 
Main block
After completing the induction block, participants proceeded to the two main testing blocks. Each trial began with a fixation cross, followed by a memory array and a delay and concluding with a probe. This sequence was identical to the practice blocks, with the exception that feedback was replaced by a 200-ms fixation cross, and the maximum response time was reduced to 2 seconds (Figures 1a and 1b). Each main block consisted of 108 trials. In the FD condition, there were 72 feature trials and 36 binding trials (see Figure 1c for the number of trials for each match state). In the BD condition, there were 72 binding trials and 36 feature trials (Figure 1c). 
Analysis
Statistical analysis was conducted with JASP 0.17.1 (https://jasp-stats.org/), and figures were made with the Python graphic library Seaborn (https://www.python-graph-gallery.com/seaborn/). Response accuracy (ACC), measured as the proportion of correct responses (“match” responses for match probes and “non-match” responses for all non-match probes, including mis-conjunct, new-color, new-location, and neither-match) out of all responses, and reaction times for correct trials were used as the dependent variables. The average response accuracy was calculated for each of four combinations made of two probe types (binding probe vs. feature probe) and two conditions (BD and FD). Subjects with an accuracy below chance (50%) for two of these four combinations were excluded from further analysis under the premise that these subjects did not understand or pay attention to the task. 
Accuracy
The Shapiro‒Wilk test showed that the accuracy data were not normally distributed; therefore, we used a data-driven permutation test to determine the significance of the accuracy data (Anderson & Ter Braak, 2003; Suckling & Bullmore, 2004). We performed a Condition (BD, FD) × Probe type (binding, feature) repeated-measures ANOVA with the permutation method. Each main effect and interaction was tested separately. For example, to construct the null distribution of the main effect of condition, the labels BD or FD were randomly permuted within each type and each subject 10,000 times. Building up the null distribution of the interaction effect, on the other hand, involved random permutation of the label across both factors within each subject (Anderson & Ter Braak, 2003). F values from the true data larger than 95% of the F values in the permuted distribution indicated a significant effect that was unlikely due to chance. When an interaction was significant, simple effects for each probe type were tested with the Wilcoxon signed-rank test and Bayesian Wilcoxon signed-rank test with 5000 permutations. 
Reaction time
The paradigm emphasized accuracy over reaction time (RT) by giving the subject a relatively long time to respond. Therefore, the RT analysis focused on determining whether any interaction found for accuracy was due to speed–accuracy trade-off. All RTs in this study were normally distributed (Shapiro and Wilk, 1965); hence, we analyzed RTs with a Condition (BD, FD) × Probe type (binding, feature) repeated-measures parametric ANOVA. If interactions were found, simple effects were tested with paired-sample t-tests and Bayesian paired-sample t-tests. In this case, the prior distribution for Bayes analysis was assigned a Cauchy prior distribution (Ly, Verhagen, & Wagenmakers, 2016; Rouder, Speckman, Sun, Morey, & Iverson, 2009), with r = 1/√2 (van Doorn et al., 2019). 
Our first hypothesis was that performance on different probe types (binding probe vs. feature probe) would be influenced by the task relevance of conjunction, which would be manipulated through the dominance of probe type within a condition (BF vs. FD block). Specifically, we assumed that conjunctions would have a higher priority for maintenance when the binding probes were more probable, whereas maintaining conjunctions would be deprioritized when the feature probes were more probable. Thus, our first prediction was an interaction between condition (BD, FD) and probe type (binding probe, feature probe), which would indicate that the task influences maintenance formats. However, because by design the binding probe was more frequent in the BD condition and the feature probe was more frequent in the FD condition, the task effect could result from having more practice with the more frequent probe (or vice versa, being more surprised by the infrequent probe). This concern led to our second hypothesis, which was that differences in the performance of the binding probe between the BD condition and FD condition would be particularly apparent for mis-conjunct probes but not for the other match states. This is because only the mis-conjunct probes critically required maintenance of conjunctions; remembering a list of all features in the array could lead to correct performance for all match states except the mis-conjunct probe if a subject strategically reacted “non-match” only when a new feature was introduced (the reader is invited to check this on Figure 1b). If the task (induced by different probabilities of probe type) affects the mode of retention beyond any practice/surprise effects, we predicted that the condition effect would be mostly found for the mis-conjunct probes. 
To test this prediction, we compared the performance in the BD and FD conditions for each match state of binding trials with paired-sample t-tests or Wilcoxon signed-rank tests when the normality assumption was not satisfied. Because we predicted no differences in some of the comparisons, we coupled this analysis with Bayesian paired-sample t-tests with a Cauchy distribution with r = 1/√2 as the prior distribution (Ly et al., 2016; Rouder et al., 2009; van Doorn et al., 2019) or the Bayesian Wilcoxon signed-rank test with 5000 permutations when the normality assumption was not satisfied. To ensure that the effect of condition on the performance of the mis-conjunct probes was greater than that for the other match states, we compared the differences between conditions (BD vs. FD) in the performances of the mis-conjunct probes against all the other four match states (match, new-color, new-orientation, or neither-match). The datasets and analysis for this study are accessible on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/w2s84). 
Results and discussion
A group of subjects performed two conditions of a delayed recognition (yes/no) task in which they decided whether a probe item belonged to a previously presented array of items (Figure 1a). In these two conditions, the proportion of binding probes and feature probes was manipulated. As only the binding probe required the memory of conjunction between features, we surmised that the conjunction between features was more task relevant in the condition where the binding probe was frequent (BD) than when the feature probe was frequent (FD). 
Effect of task relevance for feature and binding probes
A Condition (BD, FD) × Probe Type (binding, feature) repeated-measures permutation ANOVA conducted on accuracy data revealed a significant interaction (p = 0.001 by permutation, see methods section). A follow-up Wilcoxon test revealed better performance for the BD condition than for the FD condition for Binding probes, W = 205, z = 2.55, p = 0.011, BF10 = 8.32, with no differences between conditions for Feature probes, W = 117.5, z = −0.905, p = 0.365, BF10 = 0.33. The permutation ANOVA also suggested that the accuracy of the feature probes was greater than that of the binding probes (main effect of probe type p < 0.001), without a main effect of condition type (p = 0.23). 
For RTs, a Condition Type (BD, FD) × Probe Type (Binding vs. Feature) 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA revealed no significant main effect for Probe type, F(1, 23) = 0.11, p = 0.74, and a main effect of condition, with RT being shorter in the FD condition than in the BD condition, F(1, 23) = 37.32, η2 = 0.06, p < 0.001. The effect of condition was significant for both Feature probes, t(23) = −7.16, p < 0.001, Cohen'd = −1.46, BF10 = 59114.9, and Binding probes, t(23) = 4.33, p < 0.001, Cohen'd = 0.88, B10 = 119.74; however, the effect of condition was greater for feature probes, as indicated by a significant interaction (Figure 2b; F(1, 23) = 51.02, η2 = 0.06, p < 0.001). 
To conclude, these results supported our first hypothesis that performance on different probe types (Binding vs. Feature probes) was influenced by different levels of task relevance of conjunction, which was manipulated through the dominance of probe type within a condition (BD vs. FD condition). Specifically, responding to binding probes became more accurate when these probes were more abundant, and RT for feature probes was faster when these probes were abundant, without affecting accuracy. 
The effect of the task on each type of binding probe
We hypothesized that maintenance of conjunctions would be critical predominantly for correctly recognizing the mis-conjunct probe, whereas all other probes could be judged correctly by maintaining separate features and identifying each probe with new features as “non-match” (e.g., a color absent from the array) or as “match” when no new feature was detected. However, as there were no new features for the mis-conjunct probe, this strategy could result in mistakenly identifying a mis-conjunct probe as a “match.” Therefore, among all of the match states of the binding probe, we predicted that the mis-conjunct probe would be the most sensitive to the induced task. Indeed, the planned contrast showed that the effect of condition (BD vs. FD) on accuracy was greater for the mis-conjunct probe than for the other probes combined, t(92) = 2.63, p = 0.01 (Figure 2c). That is, the mis-conjunct probes benefited from the binding probes being more frequent than other match states. The Wilcoxon signed-rank test was subsequently applied to compare each match state in terms of their condition effects. Only the mis-conjunct probes elicited a significant effect of condition (Table 1a). These results also provided evidence that the better performance of the binding probe in the BD condition than in the FD condition was unlikely to be due to practice-related improvement in response to the binding probe in BD condition, or surprise-related impairment in response to the binding probe in FD condition, as all binding probes were more frequent in the BD condition than in the FD condition. 
Figure 2.
 
Results of Experiment 1. (a, b) Average response accuracy (a) and reaction times (b) for binding and feature probes from the BD and FD conditions. Here, and in other panels, small dots represent individual subject means; large dots and error bars depict across-subject means and 95% confidence intervals. (c, d) Response accuracy (c) and reaction times (d) under BD and FD conditions in each match state for the binding probe.
Figure 2.
 
Results of Experiment 1. (a, b) Average response accuracy (a) and reaction times (b) for binding and feature probes from the BD and FD conditions. Here, and in other panels, small dots represent individual subject means; large dots and error bars depict across-subject means and 95% confidence intervals. (c, d) Response accuracy (c) and reaction times (d) under BD and FD conditions in each match state for the binding probe.
Table 1a.
 
Effect of task on each match state binding probe on the ACC (BD-FD condition). BF10, bayes factor in favor of the alternative hypothesis; W, test statistic of Wilcoxon signed-rank test; z, Z-score.
Table 1a.
 
Effect of task on each match state binding probe on the ACC (BD-FD condition). BF10, bayes factor in favor of the alternative hypothesis; W, test statistic of Wilcoxon signed-rank test; z, Z-score.
As reported above, RTs were overall faster in the FD condition than in the BD condition. If the higher accuracy in the BD condition compared to the FD condition for the mis-conjunct probe was due to a speed–accuracy trade-off, then we would expect to see that the mis-conjunct probe trials were performed more slowly in the BD condition, increasing the RT difference between the BD and the FD conditions, compared to other match states. The planned contrasts did not reveal such a pattern; rather, the opposite was found—the difference between the BD and FD in RTs was marginally smaller for the mis-conjunct probe than for the other match states, t(92) = −1.745, p = 0.084 (Figure 2d). Table 1b shows the Student's t-tests for each match state. The BD condition responses were slower than the FD condition responses for binding probes in all match states but not for the mis-conjunct probes. These results suggest that the greater accuracy of the mis-conjunct probe in the BD condition than in the FD condition was unlikely due to speed–accuracy trade-off. 
Table 1b.
 
Effect of task on each match state binding probe on the RTs (FD-BD condition). df, Degrees of freedom.
Table 1b.
 
Effect of task on each match state binding probe on the RTs (FD-BD condition). df, Degrees of freedom.
To conclude, we found that memory was better for the binding probe in the BD condition than in the FD condition, supporting our hypothesis that maintaining color and orientation as separate features, or their conjunction, depends on the task. The mis-conjunct probe showed the effect of condition more than the other binding probes in the same condition, indicating that such task effect is not due to general practice. Additionally, we found that the trials in the BD condition were performed more slowly than those in the FD condition, suggesting that maintaining separate features might be easier than maintaining conjunctions. 
Experiment 2
Experiment 1 revealed a flexibility of working memory maintenance for the conjunctions of color and orientation, two features that are not location specific. However, location has a unique status. According to the feature integration theory (FIT), integrated objects are formed by tying different features to specific locations in the presence of spatial attention. Having established the flexibility of maintaining conjunctions in Experiment 1, we then asked whether a similar flexibility exists even when the critical feature is location. Similar to Experiment 1, this study manipulated the relevance of conjunctions and separate features and predicted an interaction between condition (BD vs. FD) and probe type (feature probe vs. binding probe). We hypothesized that the effect of task relevance would be observed mostly on mis-conjunct probes, which particularly require the maintenance of conjunctions. 
Materials and methods
Participants
Power analysis based on Experiment 1 suggested that 15 participants were required to reach 80% of the power with an alpha of 0.05 for higher accuracy in mis-conjunct probes in the BD and FD conditions. Seventeen students, recruited through the Hebrew University online study recruitment system, participated in the study (age range, 21–35 years; mean age ± SD, 25.02 ± 2.90 years; 77% females). Subjects with any two of four combinations (binding probe in BD condition, binding probe in FD condition, feature probe in BD condition, feature probe in FD condition) below a response accuracy of 0.5 were excluded, resulting in the exclusion of two participants. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Social Sciences Faculty at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel. The subjects downloaded the computer program made with E-Prime 3 online (https://pstnet.com/eprime-go/) to their own computers. Each participant conducted two conditions of the experiment on different dates within a week. They received either two course credits or a monetary compensation of 60 NISs (∼$15) for their participation. 
Stimuli
The stimuli consisted of spiky colored blobs, presented in memory arrays of two items (Figure 3a). The color of each item was randomly selected out of six highly distinguished colors, including red (255, 0, 0), green (0, 255, 0), yellow (255, 255, 0), blue (0, 0, 255), violet (255, 255, 0), and white (0, 0, 0). The same color never appeared twice in an array. The exact visual angle and size of the stimulus varied, as participants performed the tests on their own screens. Each spiky colored blob was limited within an invisible square of 55 × 55 pixels. The two selected locations were randomly selected out of six potential locations evenly distributed on an invisible circle with a diameter of 140 pixels centered on the fixation cross. The stimuli were created with Psychtoolbox-3, implemented in MATLAB 2018. Each probe contained only one item of the same size as the items which were presented on the memory array. There were two types of probes: binding probes and feature probes (Figure 3a). 
Figure 3.
 
Design of Experiment 2. (a, left panel) An example of a binding trial showing a neither-match binding probe. (a, right panel) An example of a feature trial showing a color-match probe. (b) Examples of five match states of the binding probe and four match states of the feature probe labeled in relation to the array shown in panel a. (c) Experimental procedure. Both conditions began with two divided practice blocks, one consisting of trials with binding probes and the other consisting of trials with feature probes. This was followed by a combined practice block, where both types of probes were presented in a random order. Each participant then completed two cycles, starting with an induction block followed by three main blocks. The induction blocks consisted of binding trials in the BD condition and feature probes in the FD condition. The majority of probes in the main blocks in the binding conditions (72 out of 96) were binding probes, and the majority of probes in the feature conditions (72 out of 96) were feature probes.
Figure 3.
 
Design of Experiment 2. (a, left panel) An example of a binding trial showing a neither-match binding probe. (a, right panel) An example of a feature trial showing a color-match probe. (b) Examples of five match states of the binding probe and four match states of the feature probe labeled in relation to the array shown in panel a. (c) Experimental procedure. Both conditions began with two divided practice blocks, one consisting of trials with binding probes and the other consisting of trials with feature probes. This was followed by a combined practice block, where both types of probes were presented in a random order. Each participant then completed two cycles, starting with an induction block followed by three main blocks. The induction blocks consisted of binding trials in the BD condition and feature probes in the FD condition. The majority of probes in the main blocks in the binding conditions (72 out of 96) were binding probes, and the majority of probes in the feature conditions (72 out of 96) were feature probes.
Binding probes
The probes consisted of a spiky colored blob presented at one of the potential locations used in the memory arrays. To answer correctly, the subjects had to maintain the conjunction information and answer whether the probe was the same as an item from the array in both color and location. Binding probes (Figure 3b) included five match states (match, mis-conjunct, new-color, new-location, and neither-match) according to their relation to the previous memory array with the same principles detailed in Experiment 1
Feature probes
Feature probes (Figure 3b) consisted of either one black item at one of the possible locations (a location probe) or a colored item presented at the center of the screen (a color probe). Subjects were required to indicate whether location probes appeared in the same location as one of the items in the memory array (location-match) or not (new-location); whereas, for color probes, subjects were required to indicate whether the color of the item appeared in the memory array (color-match) or not (new-color), regardless of its location. Thus, retention of separate features in working memory would allow a more effective response for feature probes than retention of conjunctions (which would entail a need to disconnect a stimulus from its bound location). 
Procedure
The procedure was the same as in Experiment 1 except for the trial numbers for each type of probe, and duration of the fixation and memory array presentation (Figure 3). Each practice trial began with a 700-ms fixation cross presented at the center of the screen. Next, the memory array appeared for 150 ms, followed by a 1-second blank screen, and the probe was presented for up to 3 seconds. As in Experiment 1, the two conditions (BD and FD) were administered to the same subject on separate days (within-subject design). In the BD condition, the majority of trials presented binding probes; in the FD condition, the majority of trials presented feature probes. The order of conditions was counterbalanced between subjects, and the order of trials was randomized within each subject. Each condition consisted of four block types: divided practice blocks, combined practice blocks, induction blocks, and main blocks. Different from Experiment 1, the combination of one induction block with two main blocks was repeated twice (Figure 3c). 
Analysis
The analysis was the same as that in Experiment 1. The datasets and analysis for this study are accessible on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/w2s84/). 
Results and discussion
The subjects performed a task similar to that of Experiment 1 except that the two relevant features were the color and location of the stimuli. The task relevance of conjunctions was manipulated as before by manipulating the proportion of binding probes and feature probes. 
Effect of task relevance on binding and feature probes
Condition (BD, FD) × probe type (binding probe, feature probe) repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on accuracy with 10,000 permutations (Figure 4a). Binding probes were performed significantly better in the BD condition than in the FD condition, whereas feature probes were performed significantly better in the FD condition than in the BD condition, resulting in a significant interaction, p < 0.001 by permutation, with neither a main effect for probe type (p = 0.25) nor condition (p = 0.34). 
Figure 4.
 
Results of Experiment 2. (a, b) Average response accuracy (a) and reaction times (b) for binding and feature probes from BD and FD blocks. (c, d) Response accuracy (c) and reaction times (d) in the BD and FD blocks in each match state for binding probes. The symbols and notations are the same as those in Figure 2.
Figure 4.
 
Results of Experiment 2. (a, b) Average response accuracy (a) and reaction times (b) for binding and feature probes from BD and FD blocks. (c, d) Response accuracy (c) and reaction times (d) in the BD and FD blocks in each match state for binding probes. The symbols and notations are the same as those in Figure 2.
A condition (BD, FD) × probe type (binding, feature probe) repeated-measures ANOVA on RT revealed a somewhat faster response for binding probes in the BD condition than in the FD condition and for feature probes in the FD condition than in the BD condition (Figure 4b), resulting in a significant interaction between condition and probe type, F(1, 14) = 17.03, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.03. In addition, subjects responded faster to binding probes than feature probes (main effect of type), F(1, 14) = 5.43, p = 0.04, η2 = 0.01, whereas no significant main effect of condition was found, F(1, 14) = 0.02, p = 0.88. Thus, both feature and binding probes showed faster and more accurate memory performance when presented in the corresponding task-relevant condition, which supported our hypothesis that task demand could influence the retention formats of objects in working memory. 
Effect of task relevance on each type of binding probe
We next split the binding probe trials to examine the effect of task relevance for each match state of the probes. As in Experiment 1, our flexible representation hypothesis predicted that larger differences between probes in the BD and FD conditions would be found for the mis-conjunct probe trials than for the other match states. Consistently, the planned contrast between the mis-conjunct probe and all other match states combined showed that the accuracy condition effect of the mis-conjunct probe was greater than that of the other match states combined, t(56) = 3.38, p < 0.001. The Wilcoxon signed-rank test was further applied to compare the response accuracy for trials in the BD and FD conditions for each match state. As expected, the accuracy of the mis-conjunct probe was significantly greater when the binding probes were tested frequently under BD conditions, than under FD conditions. Unexpectedly, the same effect was also shown for the new-location probe (Table 2a). 
Table 2a.
 
Effect of task (BD-FD) on each match state binding probe on the ACC.
Table 2a.
 
Effect of task (BD-FD) on each match state binding probe on the ACC.
For all of the match states, the responses to binding probes were nominally faster in the BD condition than in the FD condition, but these differences were not significant, nor was the planned contrast between the differences in the BD and FD conditions for the mis-conjunct probes and the other four match states combined, t(56) = 1.08, p = 0.29 (Table 2b). This suggests that the difference in response accuracy between match states was not due to a trade-off between speed and accuracy. 
Table 2b.
 
Effect of task (FD-BD) on each match state binding probe on the RTs.
Table 2b.
 
Effect of task (FD-BD) on each match state binding probe on the RTs.
To summarize, this experiment showed that the response accuracy was lower when a mis-conjunct probe was used in the FD condition, where the binding probes were presented infrequently, and that the response accuracy was greater in the BD condition when the binding probes were frequent; thus, maintaining conjunctions was critical. An unexpected finding was that response accuracy for new location probes was also impacted in the FD condition when conjunction information was less task relevant. In other words, when subjects were not required to report on the binding between color and location, location information tended to be lost. Comparing Experiment 1 and 2, maintaining features seemed to be easier than maintaining conjunctions between color and orientation (Experiment 1), whereas for conjunctions between color and spatial location (Experiment 2) this advantage was not observed, and in fact, the binding probes elicited faster responses relative to the feature probes, without compromising accuracy. To confirm this observation statistically, we compared the feature advantage on accuracy (frequent binding probe–frequent feature probe) and RTs (frequent feature probe–frequent binding probe) between Experiment 1 and Experiment 2. We found a significantly greater advantage for features color and orientation (Experiment 1) than for those with color and location (Experiment 2), both in the ACC, t(37) = 2.42, p = 0.021, Cohen's d = 0.36, and in RTs, t(37) = 4.14, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.41 (Supplementary Figure S1). This suggests that, in addition to task relevance, the involvement of specific stimulus dimensions may play a role in determining representation formats. 
General discussion
In this study, we investigated whether the task determines the maintenance format in visual working memory—as separate features or as bound objects. In two studies using a delayed (yes/no) recognition task, we implicitly biased subjects to maintain features or conjunctions by manipulating the proportion of probes that required conjunction information. Our results from the two studies show that both trials that required feature-level information and those requiring conjunction information were performed better when they were more frequent in a block of trials. Overall, this study provides novel evidence for the flexibility of maintenance formats in VWM based on task requirements. 
Previous studies have demonstrated the flexibility of working memory by showing that task-relevant features could be selectively retained while discarding task-irrelevant features presented simultaneously (Park et al., 2017; Pertzov, Bays, et al., 2013; Ye et al., 2016). These observations emphasized the functional purpose of working memory and challenged the view that conceptualized working memory as a task-independent system (Nobre & Stokes, 2020). Our study investigated a different type of flexibility that received less attention in that respect: whether visual information is retained as separate features (e.g., color, orientation) or as bound objects. We presented probes that required conjunction information and probes that did not, and, by manipulating the relative dominance of these two probes within a block, implicitly created a task set favoring one of the two maintenance strategies. If the retention formats were independent of the task context, performance for both types of probes should have remained the same in the two dominance conditions. In contrast, we found that retention formats were shaped by the task requirement: Performance with binding probes was better when these probes were dominant (in the BD condition) than when feature probes were dominant (in the FD condition), and vice versa for feature probes. In accordance with our expectations, this effect manifested especially in trials in which mis-conjunct probes were presented, a type of trial that critically requires the retention of conjunction information. These findings provide a new perspective for understanding some seemingly contradictory results and call attention to task requirements when investigating maintenance formats in VWM. 
We are aware of only one study that directly manipulated the relevance of binding. In their study, Vergauwe and Cowan (2015) compared RTs when the subjects were explicitly instructed to search for colors, shapes, or both. They found that retrieval of both features took no longer than retrieval of a single feature when the memory of bound objects was encouraged. Otherwise, RT increased with the number of features to be retrieved. They concluded that whether participants preserved bound objects or separate features in VWM was dependent on the testing situation. Here, we explored the binding between color and location and between orientation and color. We found that even without explicitly instructing subjects about the preferred mode of maintenance, the format of items in working memory was flexibly adjusted to optimally perform the task. 
Whether conjunction memory is required in the task is a factor relevant to all experiments aiming to investigate the object representation formats. Some paradigms have explicitly addressed this aspect. For example, to study whether a location is automatically bound to a nonspatial feature, studies usually ensure, and explicitly report, that the conjunction of spatial and nonspatial features is task irrelevant (Foster et al., 2017). In other studies, the relevance of conjunction for completing a task has not been reported but could be determined from the paradigm. For example, a study by Fougnie and Alvarez (2011) revealed that errors in memory for one feature did not significantly affect memory for other features. It should be noted that in their design, participants were asked to recall either the color or orientation of an object separately, rendering the conjunction information task irrelevant. Whether their observation would hold had conjunction information been task relevant is unknown. In some studies, the task-relevant information is subtler and cannot be directly determined from the experimental paradigm without careful attention to the experimental details. For example, in the delay-match-to-sample task in the study by Wheeler and Treisman (2002), features of different objects were not repeated within a memory array, and the probe either contained a new feature or matched one of the objects kept in working memory in both features, making it unnecessary to retain conjunctions (as explained above, this task can be solved simply by deciding whether or not the probe presents a new feature). In contrast, in the study by Luck and Vogel (1997), extended in Vogel, Woodman, and Luck (2001), maintaining conjunctions was necessary to correctly respond, as the memory array consisted of items with repeated features (e.g., two objects in an array could be red), and mis-conjunct probes were occasionally presented (where the probe was a mismatch even though no new feature was presented). Consistent with this design difference, Wheeler and Treisman (2002) concluded that features were maintained separately, whereas Luck and Vogel (1997) concluded that features were conjoined in VWM. Further investigations are required to test whether such subtle differences lead to different conclusions about retention formats in VWM. Nevertheless, the flexibility of representation formats found in the current study mandates that studies investigating retention formats should explicitly consider the task relevance of conjunctions in their design and investigate the effect of the task along with other variables of interest whenever possible. 
Our study additionally suggested that task relevance alone cannot completely determine the representation formats of objects in VWM. Rather, the degree to which an object was kept in one format or the other varied depending on other factors. An exclusive task effect would have been reflected by better performance on binding probes than feature probes in the BD condition and better performance for feature probes than binding probes in the FD condition. However, this is not what we found. For color and orientation features, the performance of feature probes exceeded that of binding probes (Experiment 1), regardless of whether conjunctions were task relevant. This finding is consistent with studies showing greater cognitive resources required for remembering the conjunction of two features not involving spatial location, including greater involvement of cortical regions (Parra, Della Sala, Logie, & Morcom, 2014) and increased impairment of conjunction memory due to a secondary task (He et al., 2020; Shen, Huang, & Gao, 2015). In contrast, when location (spatial) and color (nonspatial) features were involved (Experiment 2), we observed a different pattern, whereby subjects were faster for binding probes than for feature probes regardless of the task relevance of conjunctions. This finding is consistent with studies showing that location can be automatically encoded (Elsley & Parmentier, 2015; Foster et al., 2017; Olson & Marshuetz, 2005). Combining the results from both studies, the results confirm our hypothesis regarding the effect of the task relevance of conjunctions in determining the storage format in working memory and suggest a role for which features are to be encoded. 
Specifically, participants had difficulty identifying new locations when the conjunctions were de-emphasized, which was not the case for new colors. That is, memory of color was not dependent on memory for its location, but the memory of location required it to be yoked with color. This observation might be surprising, as the spatial dimension has long been considered a primary dimension that allows us to bind other features and to access our internal representations of objects, both visual (Kondo & Saiki, 2012; Rajsic & Wilson, 2012; Treisman & Zhang, 2006; Wheeler & Treisman, 2002) and auditory (Deouell, Bentin, & Soroker, 2000). 
One of the causes of interdependence between location memory and conjunction memory observed in the current study might be difficulty in maintaining an empty location. There is some evidence showing that an empty location (i.e., one without other associated features) is especially difficult to remember. For example, when an object suddenly appeared in a previously empty location, infants at least 8 months old were not surprised (Wynn & Chiang, 1998). In contrast, infants as young as 3 months old showed a surprise response when an item disappeared magically (Baillargeon & Devos, 2016). The above two phenomena could be explained by the inability to encode and maintain an empty location. In this case, no representation is formed in visual working memory before the object appears, and no conflict (or prediction error) occurs when it does. On the other hand, when an object is present at some locations, the memory system keeps the object bound to that location, and the disappearance of the object conflicts with the previous representation in working memory. Difficulties in maintaining empty locations have also been found in adults (Csink, Gliga, & Mareschal, 2022). 
Our study has several limitations that should be addressed in future research. Because we only applied a single delay duration in our study, we could not investigate the temporal dynamics of the process. Specifically, research is necessary to discern whether the task manipulated the initial encoding into working memory or later retention processes. Second, the formats of object representations in working memory were estimated indirectly, based on the assumption that the number of representation units (features or objects) correspondingly consumes several units of working memory resources. This assumption, however, was challenged by models conceptualizing working memory capacity as a continuous resource rather than as discrete units (Ma, Husain, & Bays, 2014). To accumulate convergent evidence and generalize our results, the flexibility of the formats should be tested with a variety of paradigms without assuming the slot model of working memory (Fougnie et al., 2010). 
In summary, our research emphasizes the role of experimental design, even implicit design, in shaping the formats of objects in working memory. It offers new evidence that the way an object is represented in working memory, whether as distinct features or as an integrated object, varies depending on the demands of the specific task at hand. These findings highlight the adaptability of representation formats beyond the task relevance of any specific feature. 
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Yoni Pertzov for inspiration on the design and discussion of the present study. We thank Areej Mousa for helping with the data collection. 
Commercial relationships: R. Cao, None; L. Y. Deouell, Innereye Ltd. (F) 
Corresponding authors: Leon Y. Deouell; Ruoyi Cao. 
Address: Edmond and Lily Safra Center for Brain Sciences, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem 9190401, Israel. 
References
An, X., Gong, H., McLoughlin, N., Yang, Y., & Wang, W. (2014). The mechanism for processing random-dot motion at various speeds in early visual cortices. PLoS One, 9(3), e93115, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093115. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Anderson, M. J., & Ter Braak, C. J. F. (2003). Permutation tests for multifactorial analysis of variance. Journal of Statistical Computation and Simulation, 73(2), 85–113, https://doi.org/10.1080/00949650215733. [CrossRef]
Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556–559, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1736359. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Baillargeon, R., & Devos, J. (2016). Object permanence in young infants: Further evidence. Child Development, 62(6), 1227–1246. [CrossRef]
Csink, V., Gliga, T., & Mareschal, D. (2022). Remembering nothing: Encoding and memory processes involved in representing empty locations. Memory and Cognition, 50(1), 129–143, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-021-01205-x. [CrossRef]
Darling, S., Della Sala, S., & Logie, R. H. (2007). Behavioral evidence for separating components within visuo-spatial working memory. Cognitive Processing, 8(3), 175–181, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-007-0171-1. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Darling, S., Della Sala, S., Logie, R. H., & Cantagallo, A. (2006). Neuropsychological evidence for separating components of visuo-spatial working memory. Journal of Neurology, 253(2), 176–180, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-005-0944-3. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Delvenne, J.-F., & Bruyer, R. (2004). Does visual short-term memory store bound features? Visual Cognition, 11(1), 1–27, https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280344000167. [CrossRef]
Deouell, L. Y., Bentin, S., & Soroker, N. (2000). Electrophysiological evidence for an early (preattentive) information processing deficit in patients with right hemisphere damage and unilateral neglect. Brain, 123(pt. 2), 353–365, https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/123.2.353. [PubMed]
D'Esposito, M. D., & Postle, B. R. (2015). The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 115–142, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
DiCarlo, J. J., & Maunsell, J. H. R. (2003). Anterior inferotemporal neurons of monkeys engaged in object recognition can be highly sensitive to object retinal position. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89(6), 3264–3278, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00358.2002. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Elsley, J. V., & Parmentier, F. B. R. (2015). The asymmetry and temporal dynamics of incidental letter–location bindings in working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(3), 433–441, https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.982137. [CrossRef]
Ester, E. F., Serences, J. T., & Awh, E. (2009). Spatially global representations in human primary visual cortex during working memory maintenance. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(48), 15258–15265, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4388-09.2009. [CrossRef]
Foster, J. J., Bsales, E. M., Jaffe, R. J., & Awh, E. (2017). Alpha-band activity reveals spontaneous representations of spatial position in visual working memory. Current Biology, 27(20), 3216–3223.e6, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.031. [CrossRef]
Fougnie, D., & Alvarez, G. A. (2011). Object features fail independently in visual working memory: Evidence for a probabilistic feature-store model. Journal of Vision, 11(12):1, 1–12, https://doi.org/10.1167/11.12.1. [CrossRef]
Fougnie, D., Asplund, C. L., & Marois, R. (2010). What are the units of storage in visual working memory? Journal of Vision, 10(12):27, 1–11, https://doi.org/10.1167/10.12.27. [CrossRef]
Fougnie, D., & Marois, R. (2009). Attentive tracking disrupts feature binding in visual working memory. Visual Cognition, 17(1–2), 48–66, https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280802281337. [PubMed]
Gilad, A., Gallero-Salas, Y., Groos, D., & Helmchen, F. (2018). Behavioral strategy determines frontal or posterior location of short-term memory in neocortex. Neuron, 99(4), 814–828.e7, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.029. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1995). Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron, 14(3), 477–485, https://doi.org/10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Griffin, I. C., & Nobre, A. C. (2003). Orienting attention to locations in internal representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(8), 1176–1194, https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903322598139. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
He, K., Li, J., Wu, F., Wan, X., Gao, Z., & Shen, M. (2020). Object-based attention in retaining binding in working memory: Influence of activation states of working memory. Memory and Cognition, 48(6), 957–971, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-020-01038-0. [CrossRef]
Hollingworth, A. (2007). Object-position binding in visual memory for natural scenes and object arrays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(1), 31–47, https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.1.31. [PubMed]
Kondo, A., & Saiki, J. (2012). Feature-specific encoding flexibility in visual working memory. PLoS One, 7(12), e50962, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050962. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Liang, Y., Pertzov, Y., Nicholas, J. M., Henley, S. M. D., Crutch, S., Woodward, F., ... Husain, M. (2016). Visual short-term memory binding deficit in familial Alzheimer's disease. Cortex, 78, 150–164, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.01.015. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390(6657), 279–281, https://doi.org/10.1038/36846. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Luria, R., & Vogel, E. K. (2011). Shape and color conjunction stimuli are represented as bound objects in visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1632–1639, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.11.031. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ly, A., Verhagen, J., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2016). Harold Jeffreys's default Bayes factor hypothesis tests: Explanation, extension, and application in psychology. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 72, 19–32, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2015.06.004. [CrossRef]
Ma, W. J., Husain, M., & Bays, P. M. (2014). Changing concepts of working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 17(3), 347–356, https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3655. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Myers, N. E., Stokes, M. G., & Nobre, A. C. (2017). Prioritizing information during working memory: Beyond sustained internal attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(6), 449–461, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.010. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Nobre, A. C., & Stokes, M. G. (2020). Memory and attention: The back and forth. The Cognitive Neurosciences, 291–300.
Oberauer, K., & Eichenberger, S. (2013). Visual working memory declines when more features must be remembered for each object. Memory and Cognition, 41(8), 1212–1227, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0333-6. [CrossRef]
Olson, I. R., & Jiang, Y. (2002). Is visual short-term memory object based? Rejection of the “strong-object” hypothesis. Perception and Psychophysics, 64(7), 1055–1067, https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194756. [CrossRef]
Olson, I. R., & Marshuetz, C. (2005). Remembering “what” brings along “where” in visual working memory. Perception and Psychophysics, 67(2), 185–194, https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206483. [CrossRef]
Op De Beeck, H., & Vogels, R. (2000). Spatial sensitivity of macaque inferior temporal neurons. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 426(4), 505–518, https://doi.org/10.1002/1096-9861(20001030)426:4<505::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-m. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Park, Y. E., Sy, J. L., Hong, S. W., & Tong, F. (2017). Reprioritization of features of multidimensional objects stored in visual working memory. Psychological Science, 28(12), 1773–1785, https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617719949. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Parra, M. A., Della Sala, S., Logie, R. H., & Morcom, A. M. (2014). Neural correlates of shape–color binding in visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 52(1), 27–36, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.036. [PubMed]
Pertzov, Y., Bays, P. M., Joseph, S., & Husain, M. (2013). Rapid forgetting prevented by retrospective attention cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39(5), 1224–1231, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030947.
Pertzov, Y., Dong, M. Y., Peich, M. C., & Husain, M. (2012). Forgetting what was where: The fragility of object-location binding. PLoS One, 7(10), e48214, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048214. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pertzov, Y., Miller, T. D., Gorgoraptis, N., Caine, D., Schott, J. M., Butler, C., ... Husain, M. (2013). Binding deficits in memory following medial voltage-gated potassium channel complex antibody-associated limbic encephalitis. Brain, 138(pt. 8), 2474–2485, https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt129.
Pratte, M. S., & Tong, F. (2014). Spatial specificity of working memory representations in the early visual cortex. Journal of Vision, 14(3), 22, https://doi.org/10.1167/14.3.22. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rajsic, J., & Wilson, D. E. (2012). Remembering where: Estimated memory for visual objects is better when retrieving location with color. Visual Cognition, 20(9), 1036–1039, https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2012.726477. [CrossRef]
Rao, S. C., Rainer, G., & Miller, E. K. (1997). Integration of what and where in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 276(5313), 821–824, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.276.5313.821. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(2), 225–237, https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.2.225. [CrossRef]
Schneegans, S., & Bays, P. M. (2017). Neural architecture for feature binding in visual working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(14), 3913–3925, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3493-16.2017. [CrossRef]
Shapiro, S. S., & Wilk, M. B. (1965). An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples). Biometrika, 52(3/4), 591–611, https://doi.org/10.2307/2333709. [CrossRef]
Shen, M., Huang, X., & Gao, Z. (2015). Object-based attention underlies the rehearsal of feature binding in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(2), 479–493, https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000018. [PubMed]
Suckling, J., & Bullmore, E. (2004). Permutation tests for factorially designed neuroimaging experiments. Human Brain Mapping, 22(3), 193–205, https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20027. [PubMed]
Treisman, A. (1977). Focused attention in the perception and retrieval of multidimensional stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 22(1), 1–11, https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03206074.
Treisman, A., & Zhang, W. (2006). Location and binding in visual working memory. Memory and Cognition, 34(8), 1704–1719, https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195932.
van Doorn, J., van den Bergh, D., Böhm, U., Dablander, F., Derks, K., Draws, T., ... Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2019). The JASP guidelines for conducting and reporting a Bayesian analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28(3), 813–826, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01798-5.
Vergauwe, E., & Cowan, N. (2015). Working memory units are all in your head: Factors that influence whether features or objects are the favored units. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(5), 1404–1416, https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000108. [PubMed]
Vogel, E. K., Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2001). Storage of features, conjunctions and objects in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 27(1), 92–114, https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.27.1.92.
Wang, B. (2017). Separate capacities for storing different features in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(July), 226–236, https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000295. [PubMed]
Wheeler, M. E., & Treisman, A. M. (2002). Binding in short-term visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(1), 48–64, https://doi.org/10.1037/0096. [PubMed]
Woodman, G. F., Vogel, E. K., & Luck, S. J. (2001). Visual search remains efficient when visual. Psychological Science, 12(3), 219–224, https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00339. [PubMed]
Wynn, K., & Chiang, W. C. (1998). Limits to infants’ knowledge of objects: The case of magical appearance. Psychological Science, 9(6), 448–455, https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00084.
Ye, C., Hu, Z., Ristaniemi, T., Gendron, M., & Liu, Q. (2016). Retro-dimension-cue benefit in visual working memory. Scientific Reports, 6, 35573, https://doi.org/10.1038/srep35573. [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
Design of Experiment 1. (a, left panel) Example of a binding trial with a neither-match probe in the main blocks. (a, right panel) Example of a feature trial with a color-match probe. (b) Examples of five match states of binding probes and four match states of feature probes labeled in relation to the array shown in panel a. The numbers represent the number of trials for each match state included in the main block of BD and FD conditions, respectively. (c) Experimental procedure. Both conditions began with two divided practice blocks, one consisting of trials with binding probes and the other consisting of trials with feature probes. This was followed by a combined practice block, where both types of probes were presented in a random order. Each participant then proceeded to complete two cycles, starting with an induction block followed by two main blocks. The induction blocks consisted of only binding trials in the BD condition and only feature probes in the FD condition. In the main block, the majority of probes in the BD condition (72 out of 108) were binding trials, whereas the majority of probes in the FD condition (72 out of 108) were feature trials.
Figure 1.
 
Design of Experiment 1. (a, left panel) Example of a binding trial with a neither-match probe in the main blocks. (a, right panel) Example of a feature trial with a color-match probe. (b) Examples of five match states of binding probes and four match states of feature probes labeled in relation to the array shown in panel a. The numbers represent the number of trials for each match state included in the main block of BD and FD conditions, respectively. (c) Experimental procedure. Both conditions began with two divided practice blocks, one consisting of trials with binding probes and the other consisting of trials with feature probes. This was followed by a combined practice block, where both types of probes were presented in a random order. Each participant then proceeded to complete two cycles, starting with an induction block followed by two main blocks. The induction blocks consisted of only binding trials in the BD condition and only feature probes in the FD condition. In the main block, the majority of probes in the BD condition (72 out of 108) were binding trials, whereas the majority of probes in the FD condition (72 out of 108) were feature trials.
Figure 2.
 
Results of Experiment 1. (a, b) Average response accuracy (a) and reaction times (b) for binding and feature probes from the BD and FD conditions. Here, and in other panels, small dots represent individual subject means; large dots and error bars depict across-subject means and 95% confidence intervals. (c, d) Response accuracy (c) and reaction times (d) under BD and FD conditions in each match state for the binding probe.
Figure 2.
 
Results of Experiment 1. (a, b) Average response accuracy (a) and reaction times (b) for binding and feature probes from the BD and FD conditions. Here, and in other panels, small dots represent individual subject means; large dots and error bars depict across-subject means and 95% confidence intervals. (c, d) Response accuracy (c) and reaction times (d) under BD and FD conditions in each match state for the binding probe.
Figure 3.
 
Design of Experiment 2. (a, left panel) An example of a binding trial showing a neither-match binding probe. (a, right panel) An example of a feature trial showing a color-match probe. (b) Examples of five match states of the binding probe and four match states of the feature probe labeled in relation to the array shown in panel a. (c) Experimental procedure. Both conditions began with two divided practice blocks, one consisting of trials with binding probes and the other consisting of trials with feature probes. This was followed by a combined practice block, where both types of probes were presented in a random order. Each participant then completed two cycles, starting with an induction block followed by three main blocks. The induction blocks consisted of binding trials in the BD condition and feature probes in the FD condition. The majority of probes in the main blocks in the binding conditions (72 out of 96) were binding probes, and the majority of probes in the feature conditions (72 out of 96) were feature probes.
Figure 3.
 
Design of Experiment 2. (a, left panel) An example of a binding trial showing a neither-match binding probe. (a, right panel) An example of a feature trial showing a color-match probe. (b) Examples of five match states of the binding probe and four match states of the feature probe labeled in relation to the array shown in panel a. (c) Experimental procedure. Both conditions began with two divided practice blocks, one consisting of trials with binding probes and the other consisting of trials with feature probes. This was followed by a combined practice block, where both types of probes were presented in a random order. Each participant then completed two cycles, starting with an induction block followed by three main blocks. The induction blocks consisted of binding trials in the BD condition and feature probes in the FD condition. The majority of probes in the main blocks in the binding conditions (72 out of 96) were binding probes, and the majority of probes in the feature conditions (72 out of 96) were feature probes.
Figure 4.
 
Results of Experiment 2. (a, b) Average response accuracy (a) and reaction times (b) for binding and feature probes from BD and FD blocks. (c, d) Response accuracy (c) and reaction times (d) in the BD and FD blocks in each match state for binding probes. The symbols and notations are the same as those in Figure 2.
Figure 4.
 
Results of Experiment 2. (a, b) Average response accuracy (a) and reaction times (b) for binding and feature probes from BD and FD blocks. (c, d) Response accuracy (c) and reaction times (d) in the BD and FD blocks in each match state for binding probes. The symbols and notations are the same as those in Figure 2.
Table 1a.
 
Effect of task on each match state binding probe on the ACC (BD-FD condition). BF10, bayes factor in favor of the alternative hypothesis; W, test statistic of Wilcoxon signed-rank test; z, Z-score.
Table 1a.
 
Effect of task on each match state binding probe on the ACC (BD-FD condition). BF10, bayes factor in favor of the alternative hypothesis; W, test statistic of Wilcoxon signed-rank test; z, Z-score.
Table 1b.
 
Effect of task on each match state binding probe on the RTs (FD-BD condition). df, Degrees of freedom.
Table 1b.
 
Effect of task on each match state binding probe on the RTs (FD-BD condition). df, Degrees of freedom.
Table 2a.
 
Effect of task (BD-FD) on each match state binding probe on the ACC.
Table 2a.
 
Effect of task (BD-FD) on each match state binding probe on the ACC.
Table 2b.
 
Effect of task (FD-BD) on each match state binding probe on the RTs.
Table 2b.
 
Effect of task (FD-BD) on each match state binding probe on the RTs.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×