The difference-with-indecision model (
Alcalá-Quintana, García-Pérez, & Miguel, 2013) provided a good fit to the data of eight observers (
Figure 3A). The proportion of the trials on which the observers used the uncertain response ranged from 2.8% to 24.1% with an average of 12.2%.
Figure 3A shows example fits for two participants in the IsoFlat (iso-oriented surround, no depth difference) and IsoDepth (iso-oriented surround, different depth) conditions. The perceived contrast difference between the reference and test gratings was estimated as the peak of the fitted the DK condition. The perceived contrast of the test grating was lower than that for the reference grating in all conditions (
Figure 3B). This was expected, because the test grating was surrounded by a high-contrast grating, which it is known to reduce perceived contrast (
Chubb et al., 1989), whereas the reference grating was presented without the surround. On top of this general surround effect on perceived contrast, we observed a significant main effect of the orientation of the surround on perceived contrast, repeated measures 2 × 2 analysis of variance: F
1,7 = 15.8;
p = 0.004. Iso-oriented surrounds were more suppressive than cross-oriented surrounds (−8.13% vs −4.89%). Furthermore, there was a significant main effect of the relative depth of the surround compared with the center, F
1,7 = 18.4,
p = 0.003, with flat surrounds being more suppressive than surrounds at a different depth (−7.1% vs −5.9%). Most important for this experiment, there was a significant interaction between the orientation of the surround and the relative depth of the surround, F
1,7 = 12.95,
p = 0.007. The interaction was driven by a significant increase in perceived contrast when the iso-oriented surround was moved to a different depth, post hoc
t test,
p = 0.013, Bonferroni corrected. There was no significant effect of moving the cross-oriented surround to a different depth, post hoc
t test,
p = 0.29, Bonferroni corrected.