The generic nasal advantage on the group level reported here is in line with that reported by
Sahakian et al. (2022) and
Mustonen et al. (2018) who used bCFS to assess a visual field effect in interocular competition. However, as noted before, this does not warrant the conclusion that there is a general nasal advantage during interocular conflict across the
entire visual field, since while there are other studies also reporting a nasal preference (
Chen & He, 2003;
Kaushall, 1975), still other studies report a temporal preference (
Crovitz & Lipscomb, 1963;
Fahle, 1987;
Stanley et al., 2011;
Stanley et al., 2019), and still other find none (
Dieter et al., 2017b). Interestingly, previous interpretations of the reported nasal-temporal asymmetries potentially offer a way out of the apparent conflicting conclusions. For example, as
Fahle (1987) already noted, in the far periphery of the visual field, a temporal bias during interocular conflict correlates with a sharper decrease in hyperacuity for the nasal versus temporal visual hemifield (
Fahle & Schmid, 1988). Fahle related these results to a greater amount of projections from the nasal hemiretina (compared to the temporal hemiretina) to the visual cortex of the macaque (
LeVay, Connolly, Houde, & van Essen, 1985). Additionally, neurophysiological results show that the nasal hemiretina contains 40 to 50% higher rod and cone density (
Curcio, Sloan, Kalina, & Hendrickson, 1990) and a three times higher ganglion cell density (
Curcio & Allen, 1990). For the far visual periphery then, a bias for the temporal visual hemifield during interocular conflict (i.e. the result of
Fahle (1987)) appears to go hand in hand with a neurophysiology supporting this. Could a nasal preference be restricted to the nearer visual periphery? This appears to be the case, it has been noted that a nasal visual field advantage is adaptive when confronted with monocular occlusion zones (
Arnold, 2011;
Nakayama & Shimojo, 1990;
Sahakian et al., 2022). As
Sahakian et al. (2022) demonstrated, when fixating a distant object that is partly occluded by a nearby object, the fixated part of the object of interest is in the nasal visual field, while the occluding part of the
nearby object (the occluder) is in the temporal visual field. Because the fixated object is the object of interest, it is adaptive to bias perception toward the nasal information, at the cost of processing the temporal information. I therefore suggest that a nasal bias is adaptive in areas surrounding the central visual field, whereas a temporal advantage is adaptive in the far periphery. Future studies could test this hypothesis by applying tCFS also in the far periphery. Still, opposing this hypothesis is the fact that
Stanley et al. (2011) and
Stanley et al. (2019) reported a
temporal bias during interocular conflict for targets presented at 4° VA and 0.38°–1.13° VA, respectively. Inspecting their results, however, reveals that the number of observers they used is limited (5 and 9, respectively). Notably, in the current study, eight of the 36 observers showed a tendency for a temporal advantage. Thus it is conceivable that the differences in naso-temporal asymmetries are (partly) the result of a sampling bias. Also, based on the fact that different studies used different methods to evoke interocular conflict (the results of
Crovitz and Lipscomb (1963),
Stanley et al. (2011) and
Stanley et al. (2019) were acquired using onset rivalry, those of
Chen and He (2003),
Kaushall (1975), and
Fahle (1987) were acquired using ongoing binocular rivalry, and those by
Mustonen et al. (2018),
Sahakian et al. (2022) and the ones reported here were acquired using (a variant of) bCFS), future studies need to compare nasal-temporal asymmetries with different variants (e.g., onset rivalry, bCFS, ongoing) of interocular conflict.